A Pilot Study Exploring the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator as a Predictor of Success in
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy
Daniel P. Hurley
Dissertation Submitted in Partial Fulfillment
of the Requirements for the Degree of
Doctor of Philosophy in
Clinical Psychology
California Institute for Human Science
September 2015
Table of Contents
Dedication and Acknowledgements 6
Proposed Value of the Study 10
Introduction to General Semantics 11
Introduction to Jungian Psychological Types 20
Summary of the REBT/CBT process 28
Chapter 2 – Review of the Literature 34
Comparison of Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT) and Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy (REBT) 34
The Efficacy of Cognitive Behavioral Therapy and Rational Emotive Therapy 37
Frustration Intolerance Beliefs: Their Relationship with Depression, Anxiety, and Anger 40
A Consideration of Automatic Processing 42
Automaticity of Higher Mental Processes 46
Attentional Bias, Self-consciousness and Perfectionism in Social Phobia 50
Clinical Implications of Cognitive Bias Modification for Interpretative Biases in Social Anxiety 52
Utility of CBT/REBT for Social Anxiety 55
Social Comparison and Well-Being 80
Mindfulness Based Cognitive Therapy 85
The Search for Meaning in Life and Well-Being 88
Individual Differences in Subjective Happiness 99
Chapter 4 – Summary of Data Analysis 106
Chapter 5 – Conclusion and Recommendations 109
APPENDIX A – Positive Psychology Questionnaires 177
Subjective Happiness Scale (SHS) 177
Personal Growth Initiative Scale (PGIS) 178
Meaning in Life Questionnaire (MLQ) 180
Meaning in Life (Presence) Questionnaire 182
Satisfaction with Life Scale 183
APPENDIX B – Informed Consent For Participants 188
Opportunities to Ask Questions 191
Opportunities to Withdraw at Will 191
Opportunities to be Informed of Results 192
APPENDIX C – Myers-Briggs Assessment 193
APPENDIX D – Presentation on CBT/REBT 204
APPENDIX E – Collected data 218
Average before and after CBT concept exposure 219
Average before and after CBT concept exposure 219
Average before and after CBT concept exposure 219
APPENDIX F – Statistical Data Analysis 221
Dedication and Acknowledgements
To my parents, Donald and Lillian Hurley, who gave me life, sustenance, and a passion for education.
To my spiritual mother, father, friend, and beloved Brother Bhaktananda who unconditionally guided me on the spiritual path.
To my wife, Rosario, whose positive regard has been an inspiration for over 35 years.
Acknowledgements
Brother Bhaktananda who first sparked my interest in the importance of words.
Rosario who gave me the space to finish the dissertation.
The faculty and staff at CIHS who were instrumental in the completion of this dissertation.
In particular, thanks to the committee chair Dr. Brophy, Dr. Dexter, Dr.Mijares,
and the director of CIHS Dr. Umansky.
And finally, thanks to all known and unknown fellow travelers on the spiritual path who have facilitated my growth…
Abstract
The research presented in this dissertation was conducted as a pilot study to find the utility of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator as a predictor of success or failure in the use of Cognitive Behavioral Therapy in clinical psychology. Each of the 16 Myers-Briggs Type Indicator categories was investigated to see whether subjects improved, remained the same, or showed deleterious effects from Cognitive Behavioral Therapy concept exposure. For each of the 16 types, a mean-matched pairs hypothesis test was applied to each of three categories (all, female, and male) to determine if there were significant differences between the pre-exposure and post-exposure to CBT related readings utilizing psychological well-being questionnaires. The results indicate that of the 16 MBTI categories, two categories (ISTJ and INTJ) showed statistically significant positive transformation (p < .01) from the CBT concept exposure in each of the subject categories (all, female, and male), and all other groups showed no improvement and no adverse effects. Additionally, an investigation into all 8 dichotomous MBTI dimensions based on strength of personality type (i.e. I >75% or I <25%, N >75% or N <25% etc.) indicated that I > 75%, T > 75%, and J > 75% demonstrated statistically significant improvement (p < .01) after CBT concept exposure, and that all other categories demonstrated no change at a 5% significance level. Future research may attempt to extrapolate the results of this pilot study by broadening the pool of subjects and investigating the numerous permutations of possibilities relating to alternative personality assessments and psychotherapeutic modalities in relation to psychological health.
Chapter 1: Introduction
Background of the Problem
Psychotherapy represents one of the most common modalities in the contemporary world for treating individuals with mental and emotional issues in an attempt to minimize unhealthy cognitions and behaviors, and to facilitate positive and productive living and well-being. Obviously, the range of problems encountered requires a broad spectrum of psychotherapeutic treatment modalities to serve the individual needs of clients. Although professional psychotherapists are generally aware of numerous theoretical orientations, most are forced either by the rigorous demands of specialization in training and/or personal predilections to focus on a narrow part of the spectrum of psychotherapeutic approaches. In fact, even if a psychotherapist were able to become profoundly proficient in a wide variety of treatment options, assisting a particular client may still prove challenging due to generalized psychological complexity.
As there is a plethora of psychotherapeutic methods currently employed to attempt to alleviate emotional and mental suffering in millions of clients, and given the fact that the success rate for each modality is highly variable, it is of paramount importance to be able to match an applicable approach to the individual needs of the client. Of the multitude of current systems—including psychoanalytic ( Haskayne, Hirschfeld, & Larkin, 2014; Poulsen et al., 2014), psychodynamic (Drisko & Simmons, 2012; Haskayne et al., 2014; Robinson & Gordon, 2011), humanistic (Forgeard, Jayawickreme, Kern, & Seligman, 2011; Neff, 2003; Nistor, 2011; Taylor & Brown, 1988), transpersonal (McCullough, Emmons, & Tsang, 2002; Morf & Horvath, 2010), emotional freedom (Fredrickson, 2004; Schwartz et al., 2002), interpersonal therapy (Hilbert et al., 2012; Kashdan et al., 2013; Kiko et al., 2012; Moscovitch & Hofmann, 2007; Sockol, Epperson, & Barber, 2011), group therapy (Hans & Hiller, 2013a; Khoo, Dent, & Oei, 2011; Koerner, Antony, Young, & McCabe, 2013; Mitchell, Newall, Broeren, & Hudson, 2013; Okajima et al., 2013), psychodynamic therapy (Enck, Junne, Klosterhalfen, Zipfel, & Martens, 2010; Querstret & Cropley, 2013), family therapy (Deblinger, Mannarino, Cohen, Runyon, & Steer, 2011; Kashdan et al., 2013; Taylor, Abramowitz, & McKay, 2012; Van der Put et al., 2013; White et al., 2010), and existential (Calhoun, Cann, Tedeschi, & McMillan, 2000; David, Matu, Pintea, Cotet, & Nagy, 2014; Steger, Frazier, Oishi, & Kaler, 2006) to name a few—Rational Emotive Behavioral Therapy (REBT) and Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy (CBT) stand out as some of the most frequently utilized and successful (Beck, & Dozois, 2011; David, Matu, Pintea, Cotet, & Nagy, 2014; DiGiuseppe, DiGiuseppe, Doyle, Dryden, & Backx, 2013; Ellis, 2013; Hans & Hiller, 2013b; Hofmann, Asnaani, Vonk, Sawyer, & Fang, 2012; Kleinstäuber, Witthöft, & Hiller, 2012; Neff, 2003; Olatunji et al., 2013; Poulsen et al., 2014; Rapee & Heimberg, 1997; Rathod, Phiri, & Kingdon, 2010; Szasz, Szentagotai, & Hofmann, 2011; Tovote et al., 2013; Wood, Fujii, Renno, Dyke, & Van Dyke, 2014). Therefore, the ability to predict the applicability and viability of this method is of vital importance in clinical psychology. To this end, this dissertation pursues a prediction method based upon the Myers-Briggs type indicator.
Research Objectives
The research presented in this dissertation is to be conducted as a pilot study to find the utility of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) as a predictor of success in REBT/CBT. As a necessary concomitant, an in depth consideration is given to the philosophic underpinnings of REBT/CBT (General Semantics) and of the MBTI (Jungian psychological types). More particularly, this study quantitatively tests the hypothesis that there exists a relationship between Jungian psychological types, as measured by the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) of personality variables, and constructive changes, after exposure to straightforward practical CBT concepts (See Appendix D), in psychological growth measured by Subjective Happiness Scale, Personal Growth Initiative Scale, Meaning in Life Questionnaire, Satisfaction with Life Scale, and Gratitude Questionnaire (See Appendix A).
Proposed Value of the Study
The purpose of this study is to test the hypothesis that there exists a relationship between Jungian psychological types and successful use of CBT concept exposure. More particularly, this research postulates that the MBTI personality assessment may be used to provide a practical, accurate guide to decide on the utility and applicability of CBT/REBT in psychotherapeutic settings. Consequently, the main focus of this investigation will be data analysis and interpretation. If strong correlations exist, clients can be guided into treatment programs more appropriate to their psychological profile type. It is obvious that any guidance as to whether clients react differently according to psychological type to treatment modality is useful. Obviously, if strong positive or negative correlations are found, this study would be of significant value to clinicians.
In order to fully grasp the historical forces behind the development of REBT/CBT and the MBTI it is necessary to give some consideration to their philosophic underpinnings: Korzybski’s General Semantics (Ellis, 2007; Korzybski, 1921, 1994; Levinson, 2010) and Jungian psychological types (Jung, 1971) respectively. After a somewhat lengthy historical exposure to General Semantics and Jungian psychological types, an overview of two highly refined particularizations of these areas is given: REBT/CBT and MBTI respectively. Chapter 2 is an in-depth consideration of previous research concerning the main areas related to the current dissertation: Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT) and Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy (REBT), automatic processing, attentional bias, the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator, happiness and well-being.
Historical Background
Introduction to General Semantics
In 1921, Korzybski, an expert in mathematics and engineering, attempted to apply the rigorous methods of science to the sphere of human psychology. In his work entitled Manhood of Humanity: The Science and art of Human Engineering (1921), he attempted to show that through mathematical philosophy and a rigorous application of the scientific method, an explicit definition of the nature of human beings, the construction of scientific ethics, and a clear understanding of the fundamental laws of human nature would be made manifest as unambiguously as the laws of physics. In fact, it could be argued that Korzybski’s fundamental thesis in this work was to explicate the possibility of the achievement of the summum bonum. He clearly argues that this potentiality is based on the fundamental capacity of humans to generalize intellectual and spiritual experience and to benefit from the culturally accumulated inherited wisdom of previous generations.
In 1933, Korzybski produced another work titled Science and Sanity that hints at educational transformation through scientifically applied linguistic epistemological principles. However, he cautioned that all of his observations were only transiently true due to the ‘time-binding’ nature of language itself: the semantic limitations imposed by the inevitable fact that language manifests in, and is constrained to, a particular moment in time (Korzybski, 1994). This characteristic linguistic caution is founded on Korzybski’s apprehension regarding the position of humanity moving forward, and reflects his concerns about the need for methods of thinking that bring about a necessary revolution in consciousness. Therefore, Korzybski’s proposed epistemological transformation subsumes such seemingly disparate realms as quantum physics, economics, sociology, organizational development, anthropology, education, politics, psychology, communication, and philosophy. Korzybski focused on advancing cognitive transformation through a basic reconsideration of logic and language, fundamental to such a broad-based transformation. This requires epistemological introspection in which all elements related to knowing, doubting, problem solving, thinking, and communication are reevaluated scientifically and objectively through introspection, observation, and the development of new theories. It also necessitates an ongoing effort to organize and systematize the vast amount of ever-increasing scientific knowledge pouring in from diverse fields to create new technologies adapted to the ever-increasingly complex environment of modern life.
It appears that humans begin to absorb language early on, possibly while still in the womb, and are constructed as voracious language machines (Adams, 1974; Fintel & Iatridou, 2003; Textor, 2009). Attitudes and values appear to be programmed at deep levels of unconsciousness, and the influence becomes so profound that it is virtually impossible to see the world as it is, but rather as it is seen through multitudinous levels of linguistic abstraction. Phenomenologically, symbolization manifests as thinking, self-talk, talking to others, writing, mathematics, problem-solving, decision-making, and a plethora of other linguistic activities. Yet these activities impose a linguistic prison, and different languages contain different epistemological traps. For example, the E-prime derivative of General Semantics (Bittencourt, 2007; DiGiuseppe, DiGiuseppe, Doyle, Dryden, & Backx, 2013; Duží, Jespersen, & Materna, 2010)asserts that clarity of thought may be somewhat realized through minimization or even complete avoidance of the misuse of the verb ‘to be.’ Korzybski analyzed fundamental language structures and concluded that identification was tantamount to semantic disturbance. However, in analyzing elementary language structures, levels of abstraction must be accounted for. This necessitates semantic evolution; otherwise, structurally unsatisfactory conclusions arise. Korzybski demonstrated that the use of the word ‘is’ leads to inaccurate overgeneralizations, such as ‘I am bad’ or ‘I am good,’ whereas in reality, so-called ‘good’ and ‘bad’ actions are engaged in intermittently by all individuals. Hence, the desirability of a linguistic methodology of transformation, such as REBT and CBT, is clear. Erroneous identifications can be restated with phrases such as ‘I see myself as good’ or ‘I see myself as bad’. This more accurately states that it is an interpretation of goodness or badness. Rather than stating something such as ‘the play was excellent,’ the E-prime version may be something such as ‘I greatly enjoyed the play.’ In this way, a more clear and realistic expression of phenomenological experience is communicated rather than an absolutist judgment.
As another example, the Spanish language improves on the philosophic implications of the verb ‘to be’ by utilizing two different forms: ‘ser’ (to indicate a permanent state of being) and ‘estar’ (to indicate a transient state of being). In English, the tendency to express hunger by the phrase ‘I am hungry’ seems radically different than the equivalent in Spanish, ‘tengo hambre,’ or ‘I have hunger.’ Certainly having something seems a lot less drastic than being or becoming something. However, it is quite difficult to transform a statement as simple as ‘the car is blue’ into a possibly more accurate assessment such as ‘the car appears to be blue to me,’ and yet the latter statement much more accurately reflects the subjective process of perception and cognitive processing. To carry out this process in its extreme version would require intense effort, nevertheless the payoff in terms of personal transformation and joy may be worth it. CBT, and particularly REBT, directly reflect the E-prime derivative of General Semantics.
It is possible that language needs to evolve in order to incorporate what at first may seem trivial, but in the long run may communicate a more rich and complex description of the gestalt of human experience. Korzybski referred to this process as ‘psycho-logics,’ in which a complete description of human reactions is more accurately described (Duží et al., 2010; Hauck, 2011). Korzybski often said, ‘a map is not the territory,’ but it certainly attempts to represent the entire territory of human experience. Humans do not simply passively process the external world, but rather actively influence experiences through semantic filters. Therefore, transformation may necessitate training in which semantic difficulties are solved by aspiring to levels transcending language itself (Kaplan & Hamati-Ataya, 2013; Korzybski, 1994). The process of perception appears to be a macro-level statistical experience in which fine details are usually overlooked. Humans seemingly perforce perceive a universe consisting of distinctly differentiated objects in space and time in a constant state of flux. This dynamic process involves a complex gestalt of feedback mechanisms and consciousness; therefore, it is vitally important that all domains of human interaction involving business, family, and personal relationships be brought to full awareness in terms of the inevitable exchanges of semantic reactions. Otherwise, individuals may be at great risk; Korzybski (1921) warns that to gain a proper comprehension of the true definition of being human requires intense scrutiny without which fundamental misperceptions necessarily produce chaos at the individual and collective level. For Korzybski, even most philosophic and psychological perspectives inevitably lead to erroneous conclusions when the scientific method is not strictly and empirically adhered to.
Obviously, General Semantics is concerned with much more than semantic linguistic manipulation and the acquisition of verbal skills to facilitate communication. It addresses itself to the entire range of human experience and existential meaning (Narasimhan, Bhaskar, & Prakhya, 2010). Korzybski (1994) clearly affirms that General Semantics is a complex direct derivative of philology, and that his work necessarily develops a generalized theory of values through an empirical, holistic, non-reductionistic consideration of the gestalt of human experience. Therefore, for the individual, General Semantics addresses itself to the complete gestalt of physical, mental, emotional, and psychospiritual through a scientific approach that is post-metaphysical. The methods derived from General Semantics and implemented by REBT and CBT can be consistently employed and are congruent with a wide variety of psychological and spiritual paths. This is indicative not only of an objective scientific analysis—although General Semantics subsumes mathematics and science—but also the subjective, phenomenological experience of the individual. In other words, General Semantics represents an ongoing effort to continuously organize knowledge to realize systems and methodologies to effect self-actualization and mutual understanding. It aims to facilitate increased awareness of the entire spectrum of phenomenological experience in order to facilitate personal growth and more optimal communication with all individuals. This requires that an amalgamation of the multitudinous branches of science be integrated into a psychospiritual theory which, among other things, includes a deeper understanding of individual reactions to words, labels, and symbols. Such a psychospiritual approach needs to be integrative, to produce creative integration and collaboration. This approach transcends mere linguistic manipulation; better communication must include the art of listening. In fact, to experience deeper levels of understanding, it may be necessary to transcend thought completely. In any case, thinking represents merely one aspect of human activity; the entire semantic reaction includes both nonverbal and verbal levels of experience in the here-and-now reality of ‘time bound’ beings.
Empiricist philosophy based science and mathematics are insufficient to address the intensity of meaning in the psychospiritual semantic reaction to the world of objects and events that includes complex conscious and subconscious processes (Hartwig, 2011; Korzybski, 1921, 1994; Narasimhan, Bhaskar, & Prakhya, 2010). Meaning, creativity, and a productive orientation seem to be profoundly influenced by affective states. Meaning itself may be meta-analyzed recursively, e.g. ‘What is the meaning of meaning?’ It is certainly possible that no linguistic structure is sufficient to address such a question. In fact, for a more complete understanding, it is possibly necessary to analyze the entire neurological process of perceiving, thinking, and learning—evaluating through neurological theory and a consideration of the dynamic biochemical features and patterns involved in self-development and the realization of a productive existence. Korzybski points out an important extension of this fact by noting that though new abstractions may replace older ones, they tend to become static. It is essential to realize that seemingly static features of the macro object oriented environment derived from ordinary perception are in reality an infinitely dynamic process involving quantum features which are necessarily beyond the scope of neural apprehension, and yet are accessible through higher order abstraction (Korzybski, 1994; Krämer, 2013). Therefore, although high order abstractions facilitate such cognitive activities as recollection, they also act as dangerous semantic reaction pitfalls. Yet without abstract critical analysis memory itself would seem to be impossible; everyday neural experience is stored via higher order abstractions and revivified through memory (Korzybski, 1994).
In the West, this extension in the rational domain is culturally taken to be superior to approaches based on feeling and subjectivity. Cultural programming dictates a ‘rational animal’ self-concept, and this is promoted as a unique distinguishing characteristic. The supremacy of rationality currently finds its culmination in the cold, passionless realm of science (Allison, 2011; Bhaskar, 2000; Hartwig, 2011; Marquis & Wilber, 2008; Wilber, 2003, 2004). Korzybski, too, severely condemns neurologically induced emotionality, while simultaneously leaving the door open to transformation (Korzybski, 1994). The external universe is ever-changing, hence everyday experience—even scientific knowledge derived from outside sources—profoundly influences emotion, evaluation, and semantic responses. This takes place through neural associative mechanisms, and yet through psycho neural training, critical judgment and psychic balance may be cultivated.
This certainly presents a ‘chicken and egg’ dilemma: Is it the case that high order abstractions and logical capabilities are derived from a primal cauldron of unconscious feelings and drives, or can higher order abstractions influence and integrate fundamental neurological processes? It certainly seems that semantic reactions are subjectively influenced by feelings related to interactions with the environment, i.e. feelings seem to precede abstractions and are highly impactful with respect to behavior in both constructive and destructive senses. Thus, it would seem that Korzybski views that feelings and neurology supersede rationality; this speaks to the existentialist view that basic humanness and meaning are predicated upon supra-rational factors such as feeling and intuition. If so, then, to progress psychologically and spiritually requires an acute awareness of the innate propensity for abstraction, and this requires a multidimensional approach to understanding the gestalt of organismic functioning. Obviously, the innate tendency is to narrowly focus on the perceptual elements in our environment through the focus of attention, or lack thereof. This perceptual abstraction severely limits the possibility of actually realizing ‘what is going on’ in the universe at large. This is a critical point of inflection: the realization that an awareness of the process of abstraction and its inherent concomitant—the abstraction—cannot represent objective reality necessarily implies that all individuals are possessed by a subjective abstract state of consciousness. Each individual seems constrained to a subjective solipsistic weltanschauung.
According to Korzybski, only scientific structural analysis can begin to arbitrate non-semantic levels of consciousness (Bachmann, 2012; Horgan, 1994; Jacob, 1995; Korzybski, 1994). Merely the fact that perception is constrained to only a fraction of the information available in the electromagnetic spectrum alone drastically influences the process of attention. Even though we accept the reality that is presented to us, we are really only seeing a little window of what is happening. Additionally, even minimally perceptible objects are in a state of constant flux, and therefore it is only possible to abstract a miniscule subset of the virtual infinity of experiences presented to us from moment to moment. Even perceptual machinery, i.e. the senses and brain, are also in a continuous state of flux. Hence, from a neurological perspective it is quite advisable to mistrust sense data and beliefs founded upon it (Horgan, 1994; Jacob, 1995). This is emphasized in General Semantics by the concept of the extensional device known as ‘ETC’ (Varasdi, 2010). The universe, which is beyond perceptual and cognitive grasp and obviously impactful on thought patterns, feelings and behaviors, is acknowledged by the open-ended admission that there must always be more of the unknown in any given circumstance. Ambiguity and uncertainty exist at the immediate perceptual level and therefore must necessarily be propagated to cognitions via abstraction.
Since most real-life experiences are multidimensional, any system that perpetuates rigidity necessarily increases the probability of erroneous perception. Not only are perceptual field elements selectively and subjectively picked, but also at every level of abstraction afterwards more elements are eliminated by means of generalization. The reduction is due to many factors including the limitations of language, vocabulary, and personal values. Hence, since all experience is filtered through subjective semantic abstractions, it seems impossible to ever achieve anything remotely resembling absolute objectivity. However, Korzybski postulates that linguistic formulations, though necessarily ambiguous with respect to reality, are nevertheless reflexive in that worldviews may be linguistically analyzed and adaptive skills formulated (Ferreira, Slevc, & Rogers, 2005; Korzybski, 1994).
With ‘territory’ representing what is actually going on in reality in the external world and ‘map’ representing abstraction from experience, it is obvious that perception generally reveals only a minute fraction of reality, and thus the subjective world is essentially an abstract construct. Reality is constructed through abstract semantic reactions that inevitably produce drastically different characteristics than the original experience. This opens the door through generalization to contradictions and distorted, even delusional perceptions. Therefore, ongoing reminders of the complexity of perception via a highly variable neurological system in which future perceptions are drastically influenced by preconceptions are necessary.
Among the major perceptual problems may be the fact that most individuals believe that Aristotelian syllogistic logic accurately assesses reality, when in fact it is symptomatic of engagement in a complex of abstracting through a hierarchy of generalizations that are ever more increasingly removed from the objective universe at large. It seems, then, that humans are inevitably possessed by semantic transformations influenced by feeling, sensing, values, personal preferences, habits of language, evaluative statements, moral judgments, classifications, self-reflexive epistemological observations, relational mathematical abstracting, past habits of perception, assumptions, and preconceived notions, all filtering and directing (or misdirecting) the abstraction process. If so, reality is partially created through symbolic manipulation.
Social construction theory posits that the meanings inferred from linguistic messages are profoundly influenced by everyday life to create and maintain self-definition by way of social interactions and communication intersubjectively (Burroughs, 2014; Löwstedt & Räisänen, 2014). Language is a crucial element facilitating cultural change and, in the process propagating values and beliefs, implicitly carries with it cognitive and emotional connotations. Hence, values and meanings are essentially communicated by way of social conditioning rather than individually decided upon. Additionally, social programming influences all aspects of our habitual roles and communication styles thereby impacting all social interactions. This effectively creates, through reification, a schism between institutionalized social roles and the underlying true self. Obviously such a process constrains individuals to restrictive patterns of cognition and conduct. An awareness of the subjective nature of such social constructs and patterns of communication is essential to psychological health and growth.
Awareness of these complex subjective elements is crucial to the realization of wisdom, self-actualization and human progress. Hinting at the difficulty for achieving a purely semantic solution, Korzybski (1994) holds that perceptual propositions necessarily involve shifting variables to multiordinal levels of abstraction, creating ambiguity. Without cognizance of this process, personal and social strife and confusion follow (Korzybski, 1994). Without in-depth linguistic structural research and an awareness of the historical process of additive language directed toward seemingly relatively unimportant matters, the fundamental verities of life might remain unaddressed. Nevertheless, Korzybski optimistically posits that through linguistic evolution, new methods and structures may be invented to approach a coherent, verifiable universal experience of life.
As words have many meanings in various contexts, certainly multiordinality applies to language. Language is assimilated through cultural osmosis: unconscious patterns of thinking and acting which ultimately manifest as distinctive individual patterns of attitudes, values, habits, and cognitions that are then used to relate to the world through observation and judgments. The roots of the influence of the psychological unconscious run deep into the neurological sphere. Therefore, it would seem that any attempt at psychological transformation must concomitantly address underlying neurological factors that influence ideas, theories, assumptions, and modes of thinking that have become second nature. Though there is a substantial overlap both psychologically and physically between and among all individuals, individuality manifests in distinct past experiences, environments, cognitions, values, feelings, and habits.
In summary, in order to effect a semantic gestalt through a conscious psychotherapeutic/psychospiritual process, it is necessary to profoundly analyze experiences, outlooks, and individual reactions with the environment. By accepting that linguistic elements cannot precisely represent external objects, it is possible to discriminate between low order abstractions and high order abstractions. This requires a drastic reassessment and transformation of modes of thinking and language usage in order to disengage from individual and cultural habits and to produce a radically transformed epistemological worldview by the use of new linguistic structures and thinking modalities. A General Semantic method that enables individuals to transcend personal difficulties through a rigorous training procedure is vital to spiritual progress. Such a method is precisely implemented in CBT/REBT.
Introduction to Jungian Psychological Types
Generally, ad hoc systems are developed based on past experience in order to navigate interactions with others with the expectation of consistent response patterns in various social transactions. This essentially amounts to an attempt to construct systems and guidelines based on previous experiences with others. Personality theories represent a formalization of this process. The foundation for Carl Jung’s nomothetic theory of psychological types derives from his work of the same title published in 1921 that endeavored to create a methodological framework for researchers attempting to categorize disparate psychological perspectives (Jung, 1971). Jung’s psychological type theory may act as a critical psychological guideline for comprehending these external portions of the human psyche. More importantly, on a deeper level Jung maintains that the human psyche requires balance. This theory asserts that since the unconsciousness, which naturally strives to complement the conscious mind, has a primitive, primeval nature and since this force cannot be dislodged from the human psyche, it requires an ongoing effort at radical transformation. Repressed tendencies retain instinctual potency and remain unconscious until an accretion of repressed energy perforce manifests symptomatically through catastrophic physical or psychological collapse; objectivity is nullified by subjective energy. Thus, the force of the unconscious can paralyze an individual in a destructive compensatory fashion, opposed to the conscious mind and nullifying any effort toward healthy equilibrium. The result can be subordination of the conscious controlling personality and its natural orientation to a productive existence. In essence, an unconscious inferior function may actually act as a conscious entity. This opens the door to psychological exploration of the unconscious due to its very intrusion into the conscious domain through analysis of the pathological primitive expressions that are characteristic of the unconscious.
To fully appreciate and effect this process, it is necessary to consider the entire system of Analytical Psychology, but to summarize, the Jungian type system accounts for the manner in which individuals perceive information, the manner in which decisions and judgments are made, and the source of energy and inspiration responsible for attitudinal orientation (Jung, 2012, 2014). The opposite poles of perception are sensation and intuition (irrational functions), the opposite poles of judgment are thinking and feeling (rational), and the opposite energy orientations are introversion and extraversion. According to Jung, various combinations of these factors constitute the dominant psychological type that is under direct control by conscious will. However, from a psychotherapeutic perspective, the activities of the polar opposite inferior functions may act as doorways to the contents of the unconscious mind, and if such unconscious compensatory attitudes are not brought to awareness, then psychological imbalance ensues. Whenever an individual allocates excessive energy in any attitude or function, inaccurate perceptions and judgments inevitably manifest (Jung, 1983). Jung advocated compensation for the development and nurturing of unconscious inferior functions and observed that since the unconscious mind is temporally past-oriented and is energized by a one-dimensional conscious orientation, the primary role is to autonomously compensate for the moment-to-moment vicissitudes of the cauldron of conscious experience (Jung, 1971b). Ideally, compensation is complementary rather than oppositional and facilitates psychic equilibrium; it is essential that highly charged unconscious forces be discharged to enable the instinctive drive toward individuation and self-actualization. Thus, from a Jungian perspective, the developed awareness of psychological types is quite useful in becoming aware of inferior, or least developed functions active in the unconscious, but this does not simply mean intellectual awareness or knowledge: it requires full integration in order to avoid the deleterious effects of one-dimensional personality development. According to Jung, a profound apprehension of the maladaptive potentials of inferior functions facilitates natural and necessary growth and adaptation.
From a more simplistic viewpoint, Jung’s theory of Psychological Types contains a personality categorization based on two attitude types – extroversion and introversion, and four functions of consciousness – sensation, intuition, thinking, and feeling. The abstract introverted attitude reflects a safe withdrawal from externality and associated power interactions. Conversely, extraverts may be negatively impacted by their positive relationship with the external objects through subjugation (Jung, 1971). Sensing (S), Intuition (N), Thinking (T), and Feeling (F) symbolize the four functions. According to Jung, sensation relates to sensual perception, and intuition to unconscious perception where active fantasies are produced by focusing on emergent unconscious elements that are being brought into the domain of conscious awareness (Jung, 1971). Hence, sensation is associated with conscious awareness whereas intuition expresses unconscious perception; therefore, these are representative of polar, compensating functional opposites. Thinking and feeling develop independently from sensation (Jung, 1971). Active thinking corresponds to logic and reason, i.e. directed linear thinking, whereas feeling and passive-fantasy thinking are related to subjective evaluation and intuition (Jung, 1971). Categorically, thinking subsumes intellectual cognition and logic; feeling is related to subjective evaluation and intuition by way of the awareness of the unconscious constructs (Jung, 1971). Essentially, intuition functions to communicate images and perceptions more directly than can be achieved through rank ratiocination. If proper attention is given to intuitive insights with sensation, feeling, and thinking evenly curbed, a constructive psychic adaptation may be achieved due to diminishing the obstructing influence of intrusive external stimuli upon fundamental perception (Jung, 1971).
Combinations of the two attitude types and the four functions of consciousness yield eight psychological type categories: Extraverted sensation, introverted sensation, extraverted intuition, introverted intuition, extraverted thinking, introverted thinking, extraverted feeling, and introverted feeling. According to the theory, extraverted types are energetic, action-oriented people with a tendency to act first and contemplate later. They would tend to know a little about a lot of different subjects, and frequently interact socially. Introverts are attracted to reflection, cogitation and solitude rather than energetic activity. Intuition and sensing are conducive to gathering information. Intuition-oriented individuals revel in abstraction and pattern recognition and engage insight garnered from the unconscious. Thinking is realized through detached, rational, rule-oriented decision making. Those with a feeling orientation make decisions through a sense of harmony and consideration of others (Jung, 1971).
Although all four functions are used in various situations, usually one is preferred and dominant, while the others act as secondary, tertiary, or least-utilized (shadow) functions. According to Jung, individuals are essentially driven by both nature and nurture toward habitual manners of cognition and action according to the dichotomous psychological attributesADDIN CSL_CITATION { “citationItems” : [ { “id” : “ITEM-1”, “itemData” : { “author” : [ { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Jung”, “given” : “C”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” } ], “id” : “ITEM-1”, “issued” : { “date-parts” : [ [ “2013” ] ] }, “title” : “The basic postulates of analytical psychology”, “type” : “article-journal” }, “uris” : [ “http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=fe13d3a1-9457-4b52-81a2-d59f07b40a2d” ] }, { “id” : “ITEM-2”, “itemData” : { “author” : [ { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Jung”, “given” : “CG”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” } ], “id” : “ITEM-2”, “issued” : { “date-parts” : [ [ “2014” ] ] }, “title” : “Collected Works of CG Jung, Volume 11: Psychology and Religion: West and East”, “type” : “book” }, “uris” : [ “http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=b7d0b1fa-be7a-4a42-96c0-f8b5a89398cc” ] }, { “id” : “ITEM-3”, “itemData” : { “author” : [ { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Jung”, “given” : “CG”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” } ], “id” : “ITEM-3”, “issued” : { “date-parts” : [ [ “2012” ] ] }, “title” : “Psychology of the Unconscious”, “type” : “book” }, “uris” : [ “http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=43b890eb-3922-4304-8bcb-b7dbbad1f4ad” ] } ], “mendeley” : { “formattedCitation” : “(C Jung, 2013; CG Jung, 2012, 2014)”, “plainTextFormattedCitation” : “(C Jung, 2013; CG Jung, 2012, 2014)”, “previouslyFormattedCitation” : “(C Jung, 2013; CG Jung, 2012, 2014)” }, “properties” : { “noteIndex” : 0 }, “schema” : “https://github.com/citation-style-language/schema/raw/master/csl-citation.json” }. As alluded to previously, Analytical Psychology proposes that the dominant characteristic resonates with the conscious mind and the opposite paired inferior function characteristic is a repressed manifestation of the unconscious (Jung, 2012). Thus, the development of secondary functions acts in concert with the opening of the unconscious, and when this does not occur, psychic imbalance develops due to the inherent stress between dominant and inferior psychic complexes.
In summary, within Jung’s typological system individuals are classified by two mental functions and attitudes into four dichotomies: Extraversion (E) vs. Introversion (I), Sensing (S) vs. Intuition (N), and Thinking (T) vs. Feeling (F). Sensing and intuition relate to the method by which an individual takes in information, i.e. sensing types receive information sensually whereas intuition oriented types primarily utilize conceptual schemata. The thinking function is related to impersonal analysis and linear thinking whereas the feeling function utilizes sensitivity to others in nonlinear decision-making. Additionally, thinking types attempt to use objective reasoning devoid of emotion in decision-making whereas feeling types utilize subjective emotion as a basis for decision-making. One’s conscious attitudinal orientation is specified by the introvert/extravert dichotomy. Extroversion and introversion relate to the source of energy that nourishes mental functioning, with extraverts being informed by external sources, while introverts derive sustenance from their own cognitive world. Introversion is reflective of the subjective orientation and is conducive to solitary cogitation. Conversely, extraversion is reflective of object-oriented, connected social activity permitting influence from the environment at large. With regard to perception and the four functions, sensing is related to present moment oriented realism, while intuition is open to multitudinous possibilities and attitudinal changes.
To generalize, Jung’s psychological type theory attempts to categorize the virtual infinity of individual psychological orientations and behaviors. It represents an insightful conceptualization and categorization for dealing with the multitudinous patterns of psychic activity. Additionally, relative to this dissertation, it provides the basis for theoretical extension by Katharine Cook Briggs and Isabel Briggs Myers who in 1962 developed the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator psychometric assessment (Myers, 1962). This was published and written specifically based on the concept that psychological problems may be more successfully addressed by utilizing Jung’s psychological typologies by focusing on the creation of an instrument reflecting both introversion and extraversion along with classifications of perception and judgment through a practical system (Myers & Myers, 2010).
Summary of the MBTI
The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) assessment measures personality types based on Jungian psychological type categories with an extension to include perception and judgment. Although it is based on Jungian psychological types, it seems to clarify and simplify individual tendencies and innate psychic proclivities. These basic differences concern the way people “prefer” to use their minds, specifically the way they perceive and the way they make judgments (Myers & Myers, 2010). Individuals derive energy in distinct ways (E vs. I), gather information differently (S vs. N), make decisions in unique ways (T vs. F), and organize the world in dichotomous ways (J vs. P).
To elaborate on each of the eight dimensions:
E vs. I. Extraverts are absorbed in their immediate surroundings, open and communicative, easily form new social connections, are genuinely interested in others, openly express their opinions, and readily accept responsibility and initiative. From this perspective, values are based on the external collective, which, even if accepted in an unquestioning manner, oftentimes leads to constructive emotional relationships (Myers & Myers, 2010).
Introverts focus on their own feelings and cognitions, enjoy solitude and reflection, form minimal friendships, enjoy maintaining personal psychic space, and enjoy working alone. Hence, even if an introvert possesses enthusiasm and constructive feelings, they will oftentimes remain unexpressed and discoverable only by those in close association(C Jung, 2013; CG Jung, 2012, 2014).
S vs. N. Sensing types are current-moment oriented, realistic, readily adapt to new circumstances, and actively pursue physical pleasures. Such types are necessarily dependent on the five senses and find such perceptual experience to be uniquely trustworthy. Conversely, for such individuals indirect ideas from secondary abstract symbolic sources, such as the written word, are less trustworthy and necessarily entail a translation to reality for meaning to be apprehended (Myers & Myers, 2010).
Intuitive types are future-directed, averse to regimented routine, enjoy abstract theory, are prone to self-reflective doubt, and are intrigued by unusual experiences. Such individuals generally relish solving unique problems, learning new skills and dislike repetition. Oftentimes energetic enthusiasm catapults them to rapid conclusions, enabling them to encounter life expectantly through conscious inspiration. Since sense impressions are repressed, imagination dominates actual observation (Myers & Myers, 2010).
T vs. F. Thinking types are logically analytical, fascinated by algorithmic structures and systems, emotionally distant, uninvolved and uninterested in facilitating social relationships. For such individuals, logic is valued over emotion and impersonal interests supersede everyday human relationships. Sterile truth is far more attractive than everyday human tactfulness (Myers & Myers, 2010).
Feeling types readily communicate emotions, tend to be ‘people pleasers’ showing deference and sensitivity to others, and are deeply involved in emotional relationships. They tend to be skilled in interpersonal persuasion dynamics that lead to harmony and agreement oftentimes through concession. Such individuals are usually skillful in personal relationships and value harmony, even to the point of making concessions to facilitate and preserve relationships (Myers & Myers, 2010).
P vs. J. Perceiving types are able to multitask without feeling compelled to completion, are not burdened by an undue sense of responsibility, are impulsive, and act based on moods without regard to necessary preparations. Perceptive types oftentimes engage in rigid, discipline-oriented parental practices creating faithful followers that mindlessly obey (Myers & Myers, 2010).
Judging types are meticulous planners, finish tasks that they have started, are rule and discipline oriented, like to “dot all I’s” and “cross all T’s,” relish stability, and are averse to retracting decisions. The perception orientation relates to the awareness of people, ideas, and external events. ‘Judgment’ is characterized by the process of arriving at conclusions from perception. They are inextricably entwined as perception dictates experience and judgment is utilized to take appropriate action. Hence, differences in style of perception and judgment produce behavioral differences (Briggs & Myers, 1995).
Overview of Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy and Cognitive Behavioral Therapy
Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy (REBT) and Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT) may seem at first to have little in common with Jungian Psychological Types and the MBTI. However, there are distinct and fundamental points of tangency between the two. Both possess clearly intrinsic connections with General Semantics. Numerous times, Albert Ellis credited General Semantics as fundamental to his development of Rational Emotive Behavioral Therapy (David, Matu, Pintea, Cotet, & Nagy, 2014; DiGiuseppe, DiGiuseppe, Doyle, Dryden, & Backx, 2013; Ellis, 2001, 2013). Additionally, he credited Korzybski for his pioneering efforts in dramatizing the importance of personal linguistic constructions by noting that imprecise thinking manifests in generalized language constructs, which lead to recursively more insidious, habitual imprecision in language habits. Ellis (2001) struck an optimistic note by pointing out that negative linguistic habits are self-constructed and therefore open to change and transformation. Also, as will be shown in the data analysis relating to this dissertation, there is a direct linkage between Jungian psychological types measured by the MBTI and the efficacy of REBT/CBT.
Summary of the REBT/CBT process
The fundamental philosophic basis of REBT/CBT is the assertion that thoughts are necessarily juxtaposed between external events and internal emotional reactions. The process may be summarized as an actual activating event producing a seemingly immediate interpretation filtering through both rational and irrational beliefs and evaluations producing behavioral, emotional, and cognitive consequences (DiGiuseppe, DiGiuseppe, Doyle, Dryden, & Backx, 2013; Ellis, 2001). Ellis (2001) believed that Korzybski held a similar notion of the process where individuals perceive an event and unconsciously construct evaluations producing internal verbalizations and emotional reactions.
Essentially, an external stimulus acts as a catalyst to an internal evaluative thought that then produces an emotional reaction. Contrary to commonly held viewpoints, the emotional response is not caused by the stimulus itself, but rather by the thoughts related to the stimulus. The REBT/CBT approach further maintains that many thoughts are distortions of reality and that individuals possess the capacity for self-awareness and modification of distorted views. Unrealistic cognitive distortions are seen as the essential cause of psychological distress and dysfunction. Thoughts such as ‘nobody ever listens to me,’ ‘I never do anything right,’ ‘I’m no good,’ and ‘nobody likes me’ are examples of the types of statements that may cause ‘hopeless’ emotions and negative self-image. Rational Emotive Behavioral Therapy attempts to deconstruct this process(Bayne, 1995; McCaulley, 1981; Pittenger, 1993).
REBT/CBT practice requires a comprehension of the interrelationship between and among thoughts, emotions, and behaviors. In REBT oriented therapy, psychological healing is realized as a client gains an awareness of cognitive distortions and reinforcing behaviors by upgrading viewpoints to more realistic perspectives. The client and therapist join together to analyze distorted thoughts and assumptions by reality testing them and engaging in behaviors that contradict the cognitive distortions (Ellis, 2001, 2013). Ultimately, it is hoped that the client is able to gain skill in the area of awareness of cognitive distortions and the modification of such distortions. Ideally, as humans are innately teachable, the acquisition of skills for the transformation of emotions via experiences with others throughout life follows as a necessary potentiality. However, this approach seems best suited to individuals who are able to introspect and apply a rationalistic, scientific methodology in the laboratory of their own psyche. As mentioned previously, an analysis of this fact by way of personality assessment is the foundation of this dissertation.
REBT/CBT, then, fundamentally asserts that individuals are able to transform their lives without the necessity of understanding unconscious motivations or why such transformations occur. Clients may simply engage in behavioral techniques acquired from a therapist and consciously alter thought patterns. Hence the focus is on becoming aware of irrational beliefs and emotions and modifying them. Thus, REBT/CBT can be viewed simply as a training program: using techniques to become aware of thoughts and behaviors and modifying them. In summary, the REBT process proceeds as follows: an activating event may feed into either a rational or irrational belief. If it is fed into a rational belief, a healthy consequences ensue and effective action necessarily follows. If, however, the activating event triggers irrational beliefs, unhealthy consequences follow, which may be utilized as fodder for rational disputation, which may act as a feedback loop to challenge the original irrational beliefs.
Some of the basic methods of REBT include transformation of many cognitive distortions such as ‘demandingness,’ ‘awfulization’, personalization, dichotomous thinking, low frustration tolerance, global rating, absolutism, selective abstraction, magnification, minimization, mind reading, overgeneralization, disqualifying the positive, catastrophizing, emotional reasoning, using ‘should statements,’ labeling and mislabeling, and all-or-nothing thinking (DiGiuseppe, DiGiuseppe, Doyle, Dryden, & Backx, 2013; Ellis, 2013). For example, mind reading is a poor practice to engage in, as for the most part none are able to truly know what is going on in the mind of another person. Yet, it can cause a great deal of distress when it is engaged in. Better to say ‘it seems as though this person thinks in a particular way,’ than the absolutist statement ‘he thinks such and such’. Using words such as always, only, or never is usually a practice that leads to misperception, confusion, and strife. Labeling represents a method of overgeneralization through lazy, simplistic thinking. If an individual says that a person is a ‘complete moron,’ it is probably incorrect, but it may severely influence any interaction with the supposed ‘moron.’
Ultimately, REBT/CBT requires an in-depth comprehension of the interrelationship between and among thoughts, emotions, and behaviors. It demonstrates methods for language modification through an intense observation of patterns of overgeneralization, labeling, demandingness, and other types of language patterns. Its emphasis on semantic precision and linguistic accuracy characterize it as a pioneering and prime practical example of a therapeutic method that implements the philosophic abstractions promulgated by Korzybski (Ellis, 2001, 2007) ADDIN CSL_CITATION { “citationItems” : [ { “id” : “ITEM-1”, “itemData” : { “DOI” : “10.1007/s10942-014-0189-0”, “ISSN” : “0894-9085”, “author” : [ { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “David”, “given” : “Oana A.”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Matu”, “given” : “Silviu A. SA”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Pintea”, “given” : “Sebastian”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Cotet”, “given” : “Carmen D.”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Nagy”, “given” : “Diana”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” } ], “container-title” : “Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy”, “id” : “ITEM-1”, “issue” : “3”, “issued” : { “date-parts” : [ [ “2014”, “4”, “22” ] ] }, “note” : “\n From Duplicate 2 ( \n \n \n Cognitive-Behavioral Processes Based on Using the ABC Analysis by Trainees\u2019 for Their Personal Development\n \n \n – David, Oana A.; Matu, Silviu A.; Pintea, Sebastian; Cotet, Carmen D.; Nagy, Diana )\n\n \n \n\n \n\n \n\n “, “page” : “198-215”, “title” : “Cognitive-Behavioral Processes Based on Using the ABC Analysis by Trainees\u2019 for Their Personal Development”, “type” : “article-journal”, “volume” : “32” }, “uris” : [ “http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=5626f7c3-0b17-4688-9bfe-1bd32e08bd73” ] }, { “id” : “ITEM-2”, “itemData” : { “author” : [ { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Oakley”, “given” : “WC”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Freeman”, “given” : “A”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” } ], “container-title” : “Clinical Psychology: Assessment, \u2026”, “id” : “ITEM-2”, “issued” : { “date-parts” : [ [ “2011” ] ] }, “title” : “Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy: Breadth, Range, and Diversity”, “type” : “article-journal” }, “uris” : [ “http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=d79fa44b-79da-42e9-a44b-c987de3231a5” ] }, { “id” : “ITEM-3”, “itemData” : { “ISBN” : “113486485X”, “abstract” : “In Better, Deeper, and More Enduring Brief Therapy Albert Ellis, the founder of Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy, describes how REBT can help clients significantly improve in a short period of time and effect a profound philosophical-emotional-behavioral change-more often that can be achieved with other popular forms of therapy.”, “author” : [ { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Ellis”, “given” : “Albert”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” } ], “id” : “ITEM-3”, “issued” : { “date-parts” : [ [ “2013” ] ] }, “page” : “320”, “publisher” : “Routledge”, “title” : “Better, Deeper And More Enduring Brief Therapy: The Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy Approach”, “type” : “book” }, “uris” : [ “http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=16da5677-347c-4fd7-bbae-c940c370ab1b” ] }, { “id” : “ITEM-4”, “itemData” : { “abstract” : “\”Extensively updated to include clinical findings over the last two decades, this third edition of A Practitioner’s Guide to Rational-Emotive Behavior Therapy reviews the philosophy, theory, and clinical practice of Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy (REBT). This model is based on the work of Albert Ellis, who had an enormous influence on the field of psychotherapy over his 50 years of practice and scholarly writing. Designed for both therapists-in-training and seasoned professionals, this practical treatment manual and guide introduces the basic principles of rational-emotive behavior therapy, explains general therapeutic strategies, and offers many illustrative dialogues between therapist and patient. The volume breaks down each stage of therapy to present the exact procedures and skills therapists need, and numerous case studies illustrate how to use these skills. The authors describe both technical and specific strategic interventions, and they stress taking an integrative approach. The importance of building a therapeutic alliance and the use of cognitive, emotive, evocative, imaginal, and behavioral interventions serves as the unifying theme of the approach. Intervention models are presented for the treatment of anxiety, depression, trauma, anger, personality disorders, and addictions. Psychologists, clinical social workers, mental health counselors, psychotherapists, and students and trainees in these areas will find this book useful in learning to apply rational-emotive behavior therapy in practice\”– \”Extensively updated to include clinical findings over the last two decades, this third edition of A Practitioner’s Guide to Rational-Emotive Behavior Therapy reviews the philosophy, theory, and clinical practice of Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy (REBT)\”–“, “author” : [ { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “DiGiuseppe”, “given” : “Raymond”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Doyle”, “given” : “KA”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” } ], “id” : “ITEM-4”, “issued” : { “date-parts” : [ [ “2013” ] ] }, “note” : “\n From Duplicate 1 ( \n \n \n A Practitioner’s Guide to Rational-emotive Behavior Therapy\n \n \n – DiGiuseppe, RA; Doyle, KA )\n\n \n \n\n From Duplicate 1 ( \n \n \n A Practitioner’s Guide to Rational-emotive Behavior Therapy\n \n \n – DiGiuseppe, RA; Doyle, KA )\n\n \n \n\n From Duplicate 1 ( \n \n \n A Practitioner’s Guide to Rational-emotive Behavior Therapy\n \n \n – DiGiuseppe, RA; Doyle, KA )\n\n \n \n\n From Duplicate 2 ( \n \n \n A Practitioner’s Guide to Rational-emotive Behavior Therapy\n \n \n – DiGiuseppe, RA; DiGiuseppe, R; Doyle, KA )\n\n \n \n\n \n\n \n\n \n\n \n\n \n\n \n\n \n\n \n\n From Duplicate 2 ( \n \n \n A practitioner’s guide to rational-emotive behavior therapy\n \n \n – DiGiuseppe, Raymond )\n\n \n \n\n \n\n \n\n “, “title” : “A Practitioner’s Guide to Rational-emotive Behavior Therapy”, “type” : “book” }, “uris” : [ “http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=71945a88-bdd4-4cd7-b66d-a63576b8284b” ] } ], “mendeley” : { “formattedCitation” : “(David, Matu, Pintea, Cotet, & Nagy, 2014; DiGiuseppe & Doyle, 2013; Ellis, 2013; Oakley & Freeman, 2011)”, “plainTextFormattedCitation” : “(David, Matu, Pintea, Cotet, & Nagy, 2014; DiGiuseppe & Doyle, 2013; Ellis, 2013; Oakley & Freeman, 2011)”, “previouslyFormattedCitation” : “(David, Matu, Pintea, Cotet, & Nagy, 2014; DiGiuseppe & Doyle, 2013; Ellis, 2013; Oakley & Freeman, 2011)” }, “properties” : { “noteIndex” : 0 }, “schema” : “https://github.com/citation-style-language/schema/raw/master/csl-citation.json” }.
Chapter 2 – Review of the Literature
This dissertation attempts to establish a connection between the MBTI and the effectiveness of CBT/REBT utilizing the following measures of happiness and well-being: Subjective Happiness Scale, Personal Growth Initiative Scale, Meaning in Life Questionnaire, Satisfaction with Life Scale, and Gratitude Questionnaire. Since no previous studies have directly explored this relationship, a literature review of the various components of the current study is explored in this chapter: CBT/REBT, automatic processing, social anxiety, MBTI, and happiness and well-being. The consideration of automatic processing and social anxiety are included to indicate a counterbalancing viewpoint to CBT/REBT and well-being respectively.
Comparison of Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT) and Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy (REBT)
For this dissertation, REBT and CBT are essentially used interchangeably, although strictly speaking, there are both differences and similarities between the two. Although REBT was derived from philosophic principles applied to psychotherapy and CBT is rooted in empiricism, both have been extensively studied scientifically through rigorous research. REBT was philosophically derived from Greek (Epictetus) and modern philosophers (especially Korzybski) as an attempt to create an empirically effective psychological therapy (Ellis, 2001, 1991; Levinson, 2010) in which emotions, thoughts, and behaviors are mixed with philosophically rooted thinking to produce an empirically founded modality; in other words, a hybrid of philosophizing and scientifically empirical approaches. In either case, both CBT and REBT utilize empirical investigation of dysfunctional attitudes based on the principle that dysfunctional beliefs are essentially self-constructed and can therefore be deconstructed. Philosophically, REBT claims that certain imperative categories of cognition, such as dysfunctional assertions of ‘should’ and ‘must,’ necessarily produce psychological problems and even that dysfunctional beliefs are synonymous with irrationality (DiGiuseppe, DiGiuseppe, Doyle, Dryden, & Backx, 2013; Ellis, 2001, 2013; Padesky & Beck, 2003). However, it does not deny the fact that environmental and biological factors may also be causative.
In both REBT and CBT, one of the primary goals is to promote introspective skills that facilitate the investigation of individual beliefs through disputation of irrational beliefs and utilization of methods for changing them (McMahon, 2011; Padesky & Beck, 2003). One of the main keys is recognition of seemingly-automatic negative cognitions, and the promotion of an awareness of underlying cognitive distortions such as over-generalizations and self-blaming. Reformulation and restructuring beliefs may be done through client self-talk, or by promoting an awareness of unconsciously held dysfunctional automatic thinking. In this way, an individual may gain a more profound understanding of the mechanics of psychological dysfunction, such as depressive cognitions or social anxiety, and develop alternatives to reduce both symptoms and the probability of relapse (Padesky & Beck, 2003). Both CBT and REBT focus on fundamentally understanding thinking processes, utilizing techniques of guided discovery, and the application of the scientific method to personal beliefs through observation, experimentation, and direct disputation of irrational beliefs (Bond & Dryden, 2000; Bond, Dryden, & Briscoe, 1999; Bridges & Harnish, 2010; Ellis, 2001; Fives, Kong, Fuller, & DiGiuseppe, 2011; Owings et al., 2013; Padesky & Beck, 2003; Vernon, 2011). In studies, both systems use behavioral experiments to test beliefs and gather evidence via real-life experiments. Additionally, both models posit that thoughts, behaviors, emotions, and physiological responses mutually interact in an environmental context, and advocate that therapists focus on the interaction gestalt in the application of treatment procedures (DiGiuseppe, DiGiuseppe, Doyle, Dryden, & Backx, 2013; Ellis, 2001). REBT tends to focus on underlying statements related to ‘should’ and ‘must’ as the basis of core irrational schemas rather than focusing on ‘automatic thinking’ (Beck & Dozois, 2011; Hofmann, Asnaani, Vonk, Sawyer, & Fang, 2012). In both systems, therapists may act collaboratively with clients via role-playing in which the therapist is simultaneously an expert guide and co-discover of meaning. However, the ultimate arbiter of dysfunctionality is the therapist acting to differentiate functional and dysfunctional cognitions (Ellis, 2001; Lyddon, Weill, & Robin, 2002; Padesky & Beck, 2003; Williams, 1994).
Cognitive conceptualizations and irrational beliefs have received empirical support as causative of many disorders such as posttraumatic stress disorder, depression, and social anxiety (Hofmann et al., 2012; Padesky & Beck, 2003; Varga, 2014). Irrational beliefs (REBT) and dysfunctional beliefs (CBT) are prevalent in many emotional and behavioral disorders (Ellis, 2001, 2013). Both REBT and CBT systems boast diminished relapse rates when compared to other therapeutic modalities (Beck & Haigh, 2014; Padesky & Beck, 2003; Waugh & Koster, 2014). However, REBT emphasizes that feeling good about psychotherapy does not necessarily equate to getting better clinically, and emphasizes fundamental philosophic transformation as key to true transformation and as a buffer against future emotional, cognitive adversity (Ellis, 2001, 2013).
The Efficacy of Cognitive Behavioral Therapy and Rational Emotive Therapy
The efficacy of CBT has been studied in relation to schizophrenia, substance abuse disorder, depression, bipolar disorder, personality disorder, insomnia, anxiety disorders, aggression, anger, chronic pain and fatigue, social anxiety, and general stress (Hans & Hiller, 2013b; Olatunji, Cisler, & Deacon, 2010). Compared to other psychosocial interventions, CBT has been found to be effective for (a) cannabis dependence in decreasing the dropout rate from therapy and minimizing relapse (Carroll et al., 2012); (b) treating smoking cessation, and decreasing relapse among nicotine quitters (Song et al., 2012); (c) problematic gamblers (el-Guebaly et al., 2012; Holst & Brink, 2010; Myrseth et al., 2011); (d) the treatment of schizophrenia when used as an adjunct to pharmacotherapy, improving mood, general functioning, and reducing social anxiety (Ciraulo et al., 2013; Jauhar, McKenna, & Radua, 2014; Naragon-Gainey, Gallagher, & Brown, 2014; Rathod, Phiri, & Kingdon, 2010); (e) treatment for depression (Beltman, Voshaar, & Speckens, 2010; Tovote et al., 2013); (f) minimizing depressive symptoms in combination therapy with pharmacotherapy (Beltman et al., 2010; Berking, Ebert, Cuijpers, & Hofmann, 2013; Cuijpers et al., 2012; Li, Fitzgerald, & Rodin, 2012; Tovote et al., 2013); (g) bipolar disorder when used as an adjunct to pharmacotherapy by attenuating bipolar disorder symptoms and preventing relapse (Batmaz, Kaymak, Soygur, Ozalp, & Turkcapar, 2013; Kurdal, Tanriverdi, Savas, Tanrıverdi, & Savaş, 2013; Schöttle, Huber, Bock, & Meyer, 2011); (h) treating anxiety disorders producing substantial positive effects when utilized as a first-line treatment approach, and in treating secondary symptoms such as sleep dysfunction, and when combined with social skills training, CBT has demonstrated superior results over psychopharmacology (Gearing, Schwalbe, Lee, & Hoagwood, 2013; Hans & Hiller, 2013a; MacLeod & Mathews, 2012; Olatunji et al., 2010; Roy-Byrne et al., 2010; Schmidt, 2012; Taylor, Abramowitz, & McKay, 2012); panic disorder (Baldwin, Waldman, & Allgulander, 2011; Szuhany, Kredlow, & Otto, 2014). CBT methods such as cognitive restructuring, exposure, and systematic desensitization for generalized anxiety disorder have been shown to be superior to psychopharmacology, and have been found to be effective in treating posttraumatic stress disorder (Ehlers et al., 2010; Farach et al., 2012; Khoo, Dent, & Oei, 2011; Powers, Halpern, Ferenschak, Gillihan, & Foa, 2010; Steiner, Petkus, Nguyen, & Loebach Wetherell, 2013; Wyrwich, Harnam, Locklear, Svedsäter, & Revicki, 2011). Also, CBT, combined with exposure response prevention for obsessive-compulsive disorders, has been shown to be similarly efficacious compared to clomipramine and selective reuptake inhibitors (Olatunji et al., 2013). Some studies show that CBT outperforms psycho-education, exposure and response prevention, pharmacotherapy treatments and behavioral stress management in treating hypochondriasis (Bailey & Wells, 2014; Muse, McManus, Hackmann, & Williams, 2010; Olatunji et al., 2014). CBT has been found to be effective in dramatically reducing body dysmorphic disorder symptoms and body image disturbances (Greenberg & Wilhelm, 2011; Prazeres, Nascimento, & Fontenelle, 2013; Wilhelm et al., 2014; Wilhelm, Phillips, Fama, Greenberg, & Steketee, 2011; Witthöft & Hiller, 2010). Regarding remission response rates for bulimia nervosa, CBT has proven substantially better than interpersonal therapy, hypno-behavioral therapy, and self-monitoring (Hay & Claudino, 2012; Hay, 2013; Poulsen et al., 2014). CBT has additionally been involved efficaciously in treatment for (a) binge eating disorder (Brauhardt et al., 2014; Hilbert et al., 2012; Iacovino, Gredysa, Altman, & Wilfley, 2012; Vanderlinden et al., 2012); (b) insomnia treatment producing favorable outcomes with respect to sleep onset latency, total sleep time, and sleep efficiency (Okajima et al., 2013; Okajima, Komada, & Inoue, 2011; Okajima, Nakajima, Ochi, & Inoue, 2014); (c) the treatment of personality disorders and antisocial personality disorder (Haskayne, Hirschfeld, & Larkin, 2014; Yakeley & Williams, 2014); (d) outpatient cocaine users with antisocial personality has met with moderate success (Drisko & Simmons, 2012); (e) anger and aggression control problems to address such problems as anger suppression and expression (Carthy, Dudley, & Ayres, 2013; Deffenbacher, 2011; Mackintosh, Morland, Frueh, Greene, & Rosen, 2014); (f) treating occupational stress (Flaxman & Bond, 2010; van der Put et al., 2013); (g) stress in parents with children with developmental disabilities when combined with behavioral parent training (Deblinger, Mannarino, Cohen, Runyon, & Steer, 2011; Moree, Davis, Thompson, & Davis, 2010; White et al., 2010; Wood, Fujii, Renno, Dyke, & Van Dyke, 2014). CBT has been found to be a superior psychological treatment for decreasing the pain intensity for fibromyalgia and chronic fatigue syndrome (Kohl, Rief, & Glombiewski, 2014; Martínez et al., 2013; Schreurs, Veehof, Passade, & Vollenbroek-Hutten, 2011; Sil & Kashikar-Zuck, 2013; Wiborg, Knoop, Stulemeijer, Prins, & Bleijenberg, 2010; Wiborg, Knoop, Wensing, & Bleijenberg, 2012). Finally, CBT has been found to be useful in controlling conditions related to perinatal depression and has been found to significantly reduce anxiety symptoms related to premenstrual syndrome (Freeman, 2010; Kleinstäuber, Witthöft, & Hiller, 2012; Sockol, Epperson, & Barber, 2011). Moderate improvement effects have been observed in treating (a) anxiety and stress for HIV positive individuals and in diminishing anger symptoms (Kemppainen, MacKain, & Reyes, 2013; McIntosh, Seay, Antoni, & Schneiderman, 2013; Spies, Asmal, & Seedat, 2013); (b) secondary symptoms related to spinal cord injury in improving coping skills and quality of life (Mehta et al., 2011; Perkes, Bowman, & Penkala, 2013); (c) irritable bowel syndrome (Enck, Junne, Klosterhalfen, Zipfel, & Martens, 2010; Garland et al., 2012; Grundmann & Yoon, 2014; Tanaka, Kanazawa, Fukudo, & Drossman, 2011; Yoon, Grundmann, Koepp, & Farrell, 2011).
In summary, although the efficacy of CBT is minimal for some psychological problems and varies by target group, it has a proven track record demonstrating that it is quite useful for a broad spectrum of psychological problems. As the research base of CBT is extensive, and given that its application is quite cost effective, the advisability of the use of CBT as a primary intervention for numerous psychological issues seems obvious. Therefore, any correlation discovered by this dissertation with respect to personality type and CBT outcome would be quite useful for therapists and patients.
Frustration Intolerance Beliefs: Their Relationship with Depression, Anxiety, and Anger
As an example of a particular application of the REBT/CBT methodology, the treatment of depression, anxiety, and anger is illuminating (David et al., 2014). In CBT/REBT in general, causality is viewed as related to frustration intolerance beliefs. Beliefs related to a low tolerance for frustration and discomfort are seen as one of the fundamental underlying causes of psychological disturbance related to depression, anxiety, and anger. In fact, frustration intolerance and self-worth are seen as fundamental underlying processes that produce, through absolute evaluation, a broad spectrum of psychological problems (DiGiuseppe, DiGiuseppe, Doyle, Dryden, & Backx, 2013). In particular, frustration intolerance is related to the reluctance to accept the differential between individual desire and reality as it is, and if self-worth is made contingent on absolute conditions, it too acts as a source of psychological disturbance. Either of these distorted belief categories can produce depression, anxiety problems, and anger (Bridges & Harnish, 2010; Fives, Kong, Fuller, & DiGiuseppe, 2011; Schloss & Haaga, 2011; Shanahan, Jones, & Thomas-Peter, 2011; Szasz, Szentagotai, & Hofmann, 2011). Hence, through both low self-esteem and low frustration tolerance, distorted beliefs can produce anger. In fact, distorted frustration intolerance and self-worth beliefs have been shown to be significant predictors of anxiety and depression, and generally, belief typology may be useful in predicting classes of emotional disturbance (Bond & Dryden, 2000; Owings et al., 2013). Frustration intolerance may manifest in many situations related to external conditions such as perceived injustice, or internal conditions such as discomfort with emotions (Bond & Dryden, 2000; Harrington, 2005). These various specific content types may be related to distinct categories of disorder. Examples of this would be represented by the intolerance of uncertainty being related to generalized anxiety and specific frustration intolerance viewpoints being related to depression.
A Frustration Discomfort Scale has been used as a multidimensional evaluation mechanism for frustration intolerance, and has been found to be psychometrically accurate in support for construct validity (McHugh, Reynolds, Leyro, & Otto, 2013; Schloss & Haaga, 2011). Statistical analysis demonstrates that frustration intolerance factors may be broken down into emotional intolerance, entitlement, achievement, and discomfort intolerance. These categories are exemplified by cognitions such as, ‘I can’t handle doing things that entail a lot of hassle,’ ‘I can’t stand having uncomfortable feelings,’ ‘I can’t stand other people getting in the way the of me getting what I want,’ ‘I need to have the approval of others,’ ‘if I do not succeed then I am a complete failure,’ and ‘I can’t handle not getting what I want.’
Many such statements indicate that demands, rather than needs, reflect a sense of entitlement related to unfairness and delayed gratification and are a feature of irrational demands underlying frustration. Also, beliefs related to perfectionistic achievement and unrealistically high standards may be particularly dysfunctional when their achievement is based on or related to self-worth (Dunkley, Berg, & Zuroff, 2012; Frost, Glossner, & Maxner, 2010).
Usually in CBT/REBT therapy, numerous psychological problems are analyzed along with a constellation of dysfunctional beliefs, and core beliefs are targeted based on theoretical assumptions as to an association between particular beliefs and specific problems. The possibility exists that efforts toward disputation of irrelevant beliefs may diminish the efficacy of this therapeutic process. It may be that entitlement beliefs are more inextricably connected to anger problems as opposed to self-worth issues. It may also be the case that entitlement beliefs focusing on life’s unfairness are little connected to depression. Therefore, the safest therapeutic route to address complex problems may be to employ a multidimensional modality aimed at frustration intolerance and self-worth issues to ensure that subtle, fundamental irrational beliefs may be addressed.
A Consideration of Automatic Processing
A seemingly drastic opposing view of cognition is considered here to give a more complete picture of the complexity involved in the extent to which individual thoughts and responses are directly related to unconscious processes. This is a fundamentally philosophical and psychological question. Some researchers have categorized styles of information processing as ‘automatic’ verses ‘controlled’ (Bargh, Schwader, Hailey, Dyer, & Boothby, 2012; Bargh, 2011; Sheeran, Gollwitzer, & Bargh, 2013). Automatic processes are unintentional, effortless, and unconscious, whereas controlled processes require effort and conscious thought (Kihlstrom, 2014). The effects of priming, or the initial activation of knowledge structures, and stereotypes arising from situational context and the extent of unconscious influence on behavior have been extensively studied (Doyen, Klein, Simons, & Cleeremans, 2014; Molden, 2014). These studies indicate that attitudes and emotional reactions may be automatically triggered situationally, i.e. evaluation, emotion and perception may be direct, unmediated psychological reactions. Attitudes may be automatically activated preconsciously, and subsequently influence thought and behavior. Some studies show that even self-concept appears to be activated automatically in the presence of relevant stimuli, and directly impacts categorizing behavior and dispositional (as opposed to situational) attribution (Howell & Shepperd, 2011; Moran, Jolly, & Mitchell, 2014; Uleman, Rim, Adil Saribay, & Kressel, 2012).
The ‘automatic’ verses ‘controlled’ dimensions have been researched in terms of attitude formation, impression formation, attribution, and stereotyping (Bargh, 2013; Dickter & Gyurovski, 2012). Perception may influence behavioral tendencies automatically, unintentionally, and unconsciously; therefore, if an individual is not aware of perceptual processing, then conscious control may be impossible. In other words, hypothetically, cognition and behavior may be automatically activated via perceptual features related to the immediate environment, and the activation of behavioral tendencies takes place unconsciously and independently of conscious volition (Bargh, 2013; Dickter & Gyurovski, 2012).
Though other studies contend that behavioral response to environmental influences is under conscious control, it is possible that conscious responses are selected based on automatic perceptions, feelings and cognitions. A hybrid two-stage model has been suggested in which an automatic perceptual reaction is followed by conscious choice influenced by individual values in which an individual may transcend automatic stereotypical influences given sufficient motivation (Massaro & Cowan, 1993; Rouder, Lu, & Speckman, 2005; Timmermans, 2012). Others investigating automaticity of attitudes, however, have hypothesized that preconsciously held attitudes are more influential in behavioral outcomes than conscious intentional ones, to the point that individuals may not be even aware of an interpretive bias, or be able to control prejudicial behavior (Devine, Forscher, Austin, & Cox, 2012; Fujita, 2011; Nosek, Hawkins, & Frazier, 2011). In other words, behavioral responses are activated immediately in a given context, and this psychological situation subsumes all possible immediate individual reactions to external stimuli as well as individual expectations for the near future, subjective evaluation of the situation, associated emotional reactions with similar past situations, and the entire spectrum of behavioral responses in an individual repertoire. This theory is born out in research that demonstrates consistent individual behavior responses over time (Mischel & Shoda, 1995, 1999; Sherman, Nave, & Funder, 2010).
Other studies indicate that conscious control over a behavior requires active monitoring of unwanted states of consciousness (Hayes, Villatte, Levin, & Hildebrandt, 2011; Winkielman & Schooler, 2011). Ironically, the probability of an unwanted behavior occurring may be increased by attempts to avoid the particular behavior (Baird, Smallwood, Fishman, Mrazek, & Schooler, 2013; Wegner, Erber, & Zanakos, 1993; Wenzlaff & Wegner, 2000). The simple act of thinking about a particular behavior may increase the probability of overt manifestation even when unintended. This has been referred to as the ‘ironic processing model,’ and may occur because the effort to ward off an unwanted behavior causes it to be more accessible by virtue of the fact that it is kept in the conscious field of awareness (Beevers, Wenzlaff, Hayes, & Scott, 2006; Wegner et al., 1993). Obviously, this is problematic if cognitions can truly be influenced by external events with a concomitant increase in the probability of a particular unwanted behavior. Therefore, active vigilance may act as a behavioral inhibitor, but such control over unwanted cognitions may necessitate concentration on something else. Some research has demonstrated that when an individual is aware of the possible influences of environmental cues and the consequent cognitive distortions, negative effects may be mitigated (Bargh, 1988; Higgins & Eitam, 2014; Kuppens & Tong, 2010). As a cognitive representation of behavior seems informationally applicable in a given situation, behaviors derive from a cognitive repertoire that may severely limit the range of behavioral options. Due to the complexity of environmental influences and potential for hidden variables, from an experimental viewpoint, it is difficult to measure the relative strengths of environmental influence on behavior as opposed to the influence of rational judgment. However, it seems reasonable that altering perception or the cognitive schema will be reflected in behavioral transformation (Cienki, 2013); hence the possibility of the efficacy of CBT/REBT.
Automaticity of Higher Mental Processes
A great deal of debate has taken place within psychology over issues such as the existence of free will and its influence in relation to mental activity, choices, and behavior (Bargh, 2013; Baumeister, Crescioni, & Alquist, 2010; Ebert & Wegner, 2011; Ent & Baumeister, 2014; Simonton, 2013). Some theorists in this area have been able to develop cognitive science without reference to conscious intention and free will while others disagree (Morsella & Bargh, 2010; Neuringer & Jensen, 2012). As with behaviorists, some cognitive theorists see behavior as deterministic and automatic. In other words, even psychological activities such as perceptions, memories, judgments, and emotions are strictly determined by preexisting causes that inevitably lead to precise outcomes. From this viewpoint, even though structural distinctions do exist in the behaviorists’ rejection of all internal processes as explanatory with respect to human behavior contrasted with such internal constructs forming the basis of cognitive science, cognitive science and behaviorism may share a fundamental deterministic unity(David et al., 2014; DiGiuseppe & Doyle, 2013; Ellis, 2013; Oakley & Freeman, 2011). Therefore, even though strict behaviorism inadequately accounts for linguistic behavior and is unable to account for the human propensity for linguistic creativity, it still coincides philosophically in terms of determinism with some exponents of cognitive theory, even though they differ by permitting consideration and investigation of internal constructs (Allen, Wilkins, Gazzaley, & Morsella, 2013; Leonardi, 2012; Vosgerau, 2010).
Even though this fundamental disparity is philosophically substantial, some cognitive theorists espouse a strict deterministic position (Schoenemann & Wang, 1996; Toates, 2006). Both behaviorists and some cognitive scientists claim deterministic evidence of behavioral and mental activities taking place without conscious intervention or choice (Bargh & Ferguson, 2000; Toates, 2006). From this viewpoint, cognitions such as pursuit of goals, interpersonal behaviors, and judgments operate without conscious will, and all constructs such as feelings, motivations, thoughts, and both conscious and unconscious impulses are deterministically caused. This represents a hard deterministic cognitive, psychological, and philosophic position. Even activities that seem to derive from conscious choice may be ultimately explainable—if analysis is complex enough,—to particular causes. It may in fact be the case that all higher mental activity occurs automatically, stimulated by external events completely devoid of any intervention from conscious will. If so, CBT/REBT perspectives may be a mere chimera.
Other theories of construct accessibility demonstrate that goals and external events may be involved in the activation of mental representations, and accessibility to mental constructs is both an additive and interchangeable (Bargh, Bond, Lombardi, & Tota, 1986; Bargh, Lombardi, & Higgins, 1988; McConnell, Shoda, & Skulborstad, 2012). Accounting for higher order processing involves motivations and goals in relation to environment. Whether these can be directly influenced by environmental situations without conscious reasoning or choice and consequently impact behavior and other aspects of advanced mentation is difficult to ascertain. Although cybernetic-like feedback from the environment involved in response activation may have influence, the development of automatic control systems in which either a self-regulating machine or an individual may be altered due to external conditions without direct conscious participation in a self-regulating mechanistic process may eventually reflect complete determinism, it is difficult to see whether a complete explanation of complex goal-directed activity can be achieved (McConnell et al., 2012). Certainly, in order for goal orientation over extended periods of time to act to guide behavioral choices, it seems necessary that environmental information feedback would be required to permit the operation of a sophisticated level of access to environmental information. Simplistic examples in the domain of engineering and machine intelligence do exist (Huang, Wang, & Lan, 2011; Larrañaga & Moral, 2011). However, the distance between human mental processing and adaptation to experience seems to be only remotely similar at this point in time. Purposive human behavior seems to require feedback and conscious intelligent goal direction to explain higher order cogitation via goal-directed motivation.
That environmental events may be able to activate internal goal structures and social information processing has been explored (Huang & Bargh, 2014; Sripada, Swain, & Ho, 2014). Yet the manner in which information is processed, stored, and remembered relative to an environmental social situation or event is not well understood. The interpretation of external events is dramatically different depending on individual state of consciousness and goal orientation. It seems likely that goal creation takes place through conscious choice thereby limiting the extent of environmentally induced automatic processes and associated judgments, evaluations, memories, and behaviors. However, it is likely that the environment, via cybernetic feedback, activates goals unconsciously. That such developments recede from conscious awareness is a likelihood in that intentional processes (such as driving a car) ultimately become effortless, operating in time in immensely complex environments. Many have argued that even automatic plans of action need to be associated with higher order goals and strategies, and concluded that habitual behaviors are connected to goal-oriented mental representations rather than environmental effects (Dijksterhuis & Aarts, 2010; Orbell & Verplanken, 2010). If an environmentally-triggered goal may seem to be set into operation effortlessly to influence behavior based on relevant environmental events due to explicit conscious choices to define the original goal, these environmental features are possibly ultimately, automatically associated with underlying goal structures. Hypothetically, then, if high-level goal representations follow lower level mental constructs and become automatically associated, then an individual may be engaged in the same behavior and goal-making due to situational features. In other words, a goal structure may become automatically activated upon the perception in an environmental situation—i.e. an unintentional skill has been acquired, analogous to being able to drive a car. This auto-motive model implies that the external events can directly trigger goal formation devoid of conscious choice and function unconsciously. This phenomenon may also occur even when impression formation is subliminally induced (Loersch & Payne, 2014; Posten, Ockenfels, & Mussweiler, 2014). This automatic activation of motivational orientation may transcend the mere formation of concrete goals and include high-level abstract motivational orientations. However, for this dissertation, such hard determinism is rejected in favor of the ‘common sense’ position that conscious will exists and may be utilized via CBT/REBT for the purpose of conscious transformation of beliefs and attitudes.
Attentional Bias, Self-consciousness and Perfectionism in Social Phobia
Social phobia provides a particular example of a prevalent anxiety disorder characterized by intense fear of humiliation and embarrassment in social situations with unknown people (Hambrick, Turk, Heimberg, Schneier, & Liebowitz, 2003; Rapee & Heimberg, 1997; Turk, Heimberg, Luterek, Mennin, & Fresco, 2005). As such, social phobia has been the focus of intense research. CBT models in particular have been explored to both understand and treat social phobia. Empirical research has indicated that social phobia derives from attentional bias towards negative social information, intense public self-consciousness, unrealistic degrees of perfectionism and expectations for social performance (Morrison & Heimberg, 2013). Cognitive bias refers to a systematic proclivity to focus on certain types of information via attentional bias, to recall particular types of information through memory bias, or to engage in interpretive bias toward ambiguous situations. Although attentional bias in threatening situations may have evolutionary survival value, the same mechanism may be maladaptive when harmless situations are perceived as extremely threatening due to a distorted belief system or cognitive schemata that produces fear. Much research demonstrates that individuals with social phobia utilize irrational attentional bias to interpret nonthreatening social information as threatening (Hambrick et al., 2003; Lundh & Öst, 2001; Rapee & Heimberg, 1997; Teachman, Joormann, Steinman, & Gotlib, 2012; Turk et al., 2005). The fear of being negatively evaluated by others may be predominantly related to the utilization of socially threatening words (Lundh & Öst, 2001).
Some social psychologists claim that healthy social interactions require both self-awareness and awareness of others (Cheng, Govorun, & Chartrand, 2012; Silvia & Phillips, 2011). Socially anxious individuals may interject negative self-referential cognitions and preoccupations producing extreme self-focus, low expectations for success, negative feedback, over-perception of the self as a central figure, negative internal attributions in ambiguous situations, and distorted perception of the averseness of the situation, all of which produces social phobia. Private self-consciousness — the attitudinal proclivity to focus on one’s own feelings, moods, thoughts, and attitudes — and public self-consciousness — the proclivity to focus primarily on outwardly public observable aspects of oneself and others — are involved in social anxiety and social phobia, with a clear direct correlation between social anxiety, social phobia, and high degrees of public self-consciousness (Ashbaugh & Radomsky, 2009; Frost et al., 2010; Hampel, Weis, Hiller, & Witthöft, 2011; Jain & Sudhir, 2010; Kiko et al., 2012; Penttinen, Wahlström, & Kuusinen, 2013).
As perfectionistic beliefs are involved in the development and continuation of social phobia, CBT has been found useful in the transformation of public self-consciousness (Brown & Kocovski, 2013; Egan, Wade, & Shafran, 2011; Frost et al., 2010; Koerner, Antony, Young, & McCabe, 2013; Mitchell, Newall, Broeren, & Hudson, 2013; Nobel, Manassis, & Wilansky-Traynor, 2011). As many socially phobic individuals hold unrealistically high standards for social performance both toward their own performance and others, modification of perfectionism seems key to alleviating social phobia and the distorted need to be perfect in order to meet public expectations. In other words, as perfectionism represents a core of social phobia, successful treatment aimed at a reduction of perfectionism and a concomitant reduction of attentional bias for socially threatening situations by means of CBT may be impactful in the diminution of social phobia. However, caution may be in order with respect to a simplistic ‘perfectionism causes social phobia’ conjecture in that individual variation may demand a more nuanced analysis of causation.
Clinical Implications of Cognitive Bias Modification for Interpretative Biases in Social Anxiety
As social anxiety disorder is one of the most prevalent anxiety disorders, has a low recovery rate, and correlates with a diminished quality of life, it warrants closer attention (Beard, Moitra, Weisberg, & Keller, 2010; Clark, 2011; Fehm, Beesdo, Jacobi, & Fiedler, 2008; Mobini, Reynolds, & Mackintosh, 2013). It is of profound importance to gain a better understanding of the underlying cognitive processes and psychological mechanisms behind the development and maintenance of the disorder. The CBT model posits that negative self-appraisals are fundamental to the development and maintenance of social anxiety by way of distorted appraisals in the form of attentional bias, judgment, interpretation, and memory (Fehm et al., 2008; Mobini et al., 2013; Moscovitch & Hofmann, 2007). It has been asserted that bias information processing is directed toward self-judgment in social situations, and that unrealistic expectations regarding social performance and an unrealistic view of an ideal self are core cognitive distortions (Neff, 2003; Spurr & Stopa, 2002). These studies have shown that the main characteristic feeding social anxiety is the fear that perceived deficiencies will lead to social exposure and shame. Those with social anxiety tend to interpret social interactions through dysfunctional assumptions resulting in the perception of social danger. In this process, anxious individuals tend to focus on negative information about themselves and create negative self-evaluations and predictions about poor future social performance through elevated attention to internal cues that trigger anxiety.
Other theoretical models of social anxiety propose that negatively biased cognitions of self and perceived negative evaluation are not the primary causes of social anxiety, rather that socially anxious individuals are primarily focused on internal cues (Mansell, Clark, & Ehlers, 2003; Mobini et al., 2013; Pineles & Mineka, 2005; Rapee & Heimberg, 1997). From this viewpoint, socially anxious individuals simultaneously process both internal and external cues to imagine negative evaluations made by others from external stimuli such as facial expressions. This model presumes that vigilance toward negative social cues is a primary element that feeds into negative self-focus, creating an erroneous belief in negative views of self by others. If this is true, this implies that changes in the environment powerfully impact social anxiousness, as individual attention oscillates between self-image and perceived external threats, creating a negative interdependence spiral of external and internal cues. Therefore, the fundamental view of the CBT/REBT model of social anxiety that socially anxious individuals are vulnerable due to a distorted information-processing bias can be profoundly useful in transformation. Otherwise, with exposure to ambiguous social situations, socially anxious individuals will exhibit negative cognitive biases. As most real life social situations provide ambiguous cues with multitudinous possibilities for interpretation, socially anxious individuals are constantly ‘at risk’ for misinterpreting innocuous cues as criticism or rejection and perceiving their own actions as inferior to those of others. The constructive resolution of ambiguity is therefore a main focus of CBT therapy, with the goal of modification of negative cognitive biases and a corresponding reduction of social anxiety.
The CBT models postulate a diverse range of cognitive processes leading to negative information processing biases and anxiety disorders. The resolution of ambiguity in social situations is fundamental to research as socially anxious individuals interpret social events negatively (Amin, Foa, & Coles, 1998; Bachmann, 2012; Beatty, 1988; Hoppitt, Mathews, Yiend, & Mackintosh, 2010; Moscovitch & Hofmann, 2007; Taylor & Brown, 1988). Research indicates that socially anxious individuals interpret ambiguous situations as more threatening than non-anxious individuals, and engage in catastrophization of even minutely negative social events (Heimberg, 2002; Mobini et al., 2013; Riskind et al., 2013). This tendency to catastrophizing leads to both negative bias and diminished positive bias in the creation and selection of responses when confronted with ambiguity (Mobini et al., 2013; Stopa & Clark, 2000; White, Suway, Pine, Bar-Haim, & Fox, 2011). This in turn leads socially anxious individuals to increased threat interpretation bias and diminished constructive interpretation bias (Amir, Taylor, & Donohue, 2011; Beard & Amir, 2010; Hallion & Ruscio, 2011; Mobini et al., 2013).
In relation to the perception of threats from social cues, individuals estimate the cost-benefit and probability of negative events occurring, with socially anxious individuals overestimating social danger and miscalculating probabilities related to negative social events by overestimating the likelihood and cost of these events (Chen, Rapee, & Abbott, 2013; Heimberg, 2002). This judgmental bias by socially anxious individuals has been extensively researched. In both clinical and non-clinical settings socially anxious individuals manifest distorted interpretations of ambiguous social situations and such interpretation biases are predominately unrecognized by socially anxious individuals (Mobini et al., 2013; Vassilopoulos, Moberly, & Douratsou, 2012). In summary, socially anxious individuals tend to catastrophize minimally negative social events, overestimate the likelihood and cost in potentially negative social situations, and produce social anxiety by way of negative self-processing.
Utility of CBT/REBT for Social Anxiety
CBT has developed numerous interventions for the modification of cognitive biases associated with anxiety disorders wherein individuals can be trained to generate positive interpretations of ambiguous stimuli; such training has produced constructive transformations in state anxiety (Amir, Taylor, & Donohue, 2011; Beard, 2011; Hertel & Mathews, 2011; MacLeod & Mathews, 2012; Viana & Gratz, 2012). Modifications in interpretation bias endure in spite of changes in the context of experimental training, and studies show that when individuals are exposed to training in benign constructive interpretation bias, diminished anxiety is experienced (Hertel & Brozovich, 2010; Holmes & Mathews, 2010; Tran, Hertel, & Joormann, 2011). When subjects are trained to interpret ambiguous situations in either positive or negative self-relevant ways, corresponding changes in self-reported affect and self-esteem occur under emotionally vulnerable conditions, especially when subjects are encouraged to consciously generate positive interpretations of ambiguous situations (Mobini et al., 2013; Tran, Siemer, & Joormann, 2011). When subjects are actively trained to select and generate threatening or nonthreatening meanings of ambiguous events, subsequent self-ratings of emotionality are congruent with the direction of training (Hoppitt et al., 2010). Socially anxious individuals who receive positive interpretation induction generate more benign interpretations in ambiguous social situations, and are therefore substantially less anxious in similar future social situations, significantly endorse fewer threat interpretations, and demonstrate more benign interpretations (Mobini et al., 2013). Therefore, active methods of interpretation training produce new attitudes in a training-congruent direction. Interpretation and attentional bias are both the targets for treatment of anxiety disorders, and treatments combining these two approaches are efficacious in significantly reducing social anxiety symptoms in relation to interpretative and attentional bias (Brosan, Hoppitt, Shelfer, Sillence, & Mackintosh, 2011; Mobini et al., 2013). A substantial body of literature demonstrates that cognitive bias modification is a highly efficacious intervention for transforming negative interpretation bias, thereby reducing anxiety disorders in general, emotional vulnerability, and social anxiety in particular (Amir, Taylor, & Donohue, 2011; Beard, 2011; Brosan, Hoppitt, Shelfer, Sillence, & Mackintosh, 2011; Hallion & Ruscio, 2011; Hoppitt, Mathews, Yiend, & Mackintosh, 2010; Lothmann, Holmes, Chan, & Lau, 2011; MacLeod & Mathews, 2012; Mobini, Reynolds, & Mackintosh, 2013).
The discovery that CBT training is efficacious in modifying negative interpretative biases in individuals with social anxiety has both clinical and theoretical implications. The research results reviewed above lend support to CBT-oriented theories regarding the causes of social anxiety and the emphasis on biased information processing producing negative evaluation with associated perceived negative self-processing inclining individuals to vulnerability in the development of social anxiety (Brosan et al., 2011; Hallion & Ruscio, 2011; Heimberg, 2002). As positive interpretation training enhances positive mood, any investigation targeting the efficacy for particular groups of individuals is highly desirable. For example, some studies (Blackwell & Holmes, 2010; Lothmann, Holmes, Chan, & Lau, 2011) have found that positive interpretation training was more impactful among some groups using mental imagery as opposed to verbal processing. In other words, images rather than verbal representations may influence positive or negative affectivity among some groups, and that may necessitate the employment of visual prompts and images during training for those who find mental imagery more accessible than verbal processing and manipulation. Therefore, generalized CBT methods for addressing a broad spectrum of psychological maladies may be more applicable to some groups than others. Hence, the focus of this dissertation to ascertain whether certain groups identified by the Myers-Briggs type indicator instrument are more prone to positive transformation through CBT methods than others appears quite useful. Ultimately, content-specific training materials tailored for specific Myers-Briggs typology groups may utilize theoretically specific clinical presentations and programs tailored to individual psychological types.
Myers-Briggs Type Indicator
Life satisfaction and psychological well-being are not just related to individual life circumstances. In fact, they may be predominantly related to personality type and attendant inherent proclivities toward the experience of positive and negative emotions. Generally speaking, cognitive styles have been the subject of numerous studies and have been defined as the intersection between independent, overlapping constructs: cognition and personality (Evans, Richardson, & Waring, 2013; Van Hiel, Onraet, & De Pauw, 2010). Additionally, numerous studies have examined the relationship between the Myers-Briggs type indicator and cognitive style, mental ability, and learning style (Carlson, 1980; Carlson & Levy, 1973; Cools & Van den Broeck, 2007; Edwards, Lanning, & Hooker, 2002; Edwards, 2003). Studies indicate that extraversion is correlated with positive affect, higher levels of psychological well-being and high life satisfaction, whereas neuroticism is correlated with lower levels of life satisfaction (Costa & McCrae, 1980; Wilt, Noftle, Fleeson, & Spain, 2012). Extroversion has also been shown to be substantially correlated with constructive relationships, personal growth, and self-acceptance (Crocker, 2011; Morf & Horvath, 2010; Schlenker & Weigold, 1990). Intuitive type personalities positively correlate with openness, feeling types correlate with agreeableness, and judging types positively correlate with conscientiousness (Furnham, Moutafi, & Crump, 2003; Furnham & Moutafi, 2012; McCrae, & Costa, 1992; McCrae & Costa, 1997; McCrae, 2013). The Myers-Briggs type combination category JP has been as associated with analytic strategies, the SN configuration with openness to inferential judgments and the TF typology has been related to analytical processing (Beyler & Schmeck, 1992; Cools et al., 2007).
The Myers-Briggs type indicator provides substantial self-understanding without moral stigmatization (Bayne, 1995; Lloyd, 2007, 2012a). However, psychological type in itself may in fact become an implicit religion and lead individuals to complacency with being what they are (Lloyd, 2007, 2012a, 2012b). This has been addressed extensively (Lloyd, 2012b; Myers, McCaulley, & Most, 1998), and the position has been advocated that completion of one’s personality through the development of least-preferred processes may be challenging, yet rewarding in later life, as alternatively individuals may retreat inappropriately into a stagnant comfort zone leading to a rigid commitment to dominant psychological type processes. Hence, a clear awareness of psychological type proclivities may be advantageous to personal growth. Polar opposites and imbalances may need to be addressed, not just in later life, but whenever they are discovered.
As the Myers-Briggs type indicator is the predominant measure of Jungian psychological types, and is one of the most prevalent psychological assessments to measure personality types in both practical applications and research, it has been the focus of copious research scrutiny (Kelly & Jugovic, 2001; Lloyd, 2012b; Myers et al., 1998; Pulver & Kelly, 2008; Salter, Evans, & Forney, 2006), and has demonstrated internal consistency with respect to homogeneity in responses and temporal stability (Capraro & Capraro, 2002; King et al., 1999; Lloyd, 2012b; Pulver & Kelly, 2008). Historically, the MBTI derived from Jung’s dichotomous psychological type theory by profiling individuals on four scales: Extraversion-Introversion (E-I), Sensing-Intuition (S-N), Thinking-Feeling (T-F), and Judging-Perceiving (J-P), resulting in 16 possible profiles such as INTJ. Additionally, a quantitative measure called the Preferences Clarity Index, or PCI, provides a numeric indication of measurement precision yielding profiles such as I-20, N-15, T-3, J-20 (Myers et al., 1998). This provides a more accurate basis for assertion of MBTI categories and strengths of categorizations. Jung theorized that the innate tendencies demonstrated by psychological types fluctuate somewhat over an individual’s life time (Jung & Campbell, 1971; Jung, 1971), and therefore the MBTI, along with the PCI tend to evaluate personality type at a particular instant in time. Hence, particular categorizations are somewhat transient and therefore testing and retesting may be necessary to provide a reflection of this fundamental continuous fluctuation of personality preferences. However, sophisticated analytical methods such as configural frequency analysis and log-linear analysis indicate that Jungian dichotomies and categorical scores of the MBTI demonstrate innate personality dispositions and stable patterns of scores (Kelly & Jugovic, 2001; Myers et al., 1998; Pulver & Kelly, 2008; Salter et al., 2006). Multiple administrations of the MBTI through longitudinal studies indicate highly statistically significant stability for both categorical and numerical scores. Therefore, even though some researchers (Arnau, Green, & Rosen, 2003; Deng, Liu, Li, & Hu, 2013; Vacha Haase & Thompson, 2002) have advocated the use of alternative measures for Jungian personality types, most researchers and practitioners have found that the fundamental personality constructs measured by the MBTI are truly dichotomous and stable. Of course, care must be taken that the MBTI is ethically administered by qualified professionals to facilitate the acquisition of increased self-awareness through an understanding of personality type disposition. In this way, awareness of personality traits and their expression based on experience and personal development, though categorically reflecting subconscious preferences toward particular behaviors, can constructively inform psychotherapeutic counseling, research, and personal development. Hence, though personality research is not a precise science and numerous theories have been employed to explain the infinite complexity of human behavior in a variety of complex situations, the theory of Jungian psychological types and the MBTI indicate that it is a valid tool to facilitate self-transformation and self-understanding.
Happiness and Well-Being
Since this dissertation attempts to measure changes in well-being after exposure to CBT/REBT concepts, and because aside from the negative aspects of personality traits and social anxiety, one of the major goals of CBT/REBT is the realization of positive states of being, an in-depth consideration of happiness and well-being is in order.
Generally, subjective well-being reflects the level of constructive feelings and thoughts that an individual has about his or her life. Cognitively, this subsumes a generalized sense of satisfaction with aspects of life, and emotionally, individuals with high subjective well-being primarily experience pleasant affect mostly due to constructive appraisals of external events (Caunt, Franklin, Brodaty, & Brodaty, 2013; Lyubomirsky, Dickerhoof, Boehm, & Sheldon, 2011; Lyubomirsky, 2001; Quevedo, Abella, Marrero Quevedo, & Carballeira Abella, 2011). Subjective well-being is highly correlated with absence of negative affect, the presence of positive affect, and general satisfaction with life. Conversely, individuals with low subjective well-being evaluate their lives and events as undesirable, which leads to negative emotional states including depression, anxiety, and anger (Lamers, Westerhof, Kovács, & Bohlmeijer, 2012; Robinson & Gordon, 2011; Wasylkiw, Fabrigar, Rainboth, Reid, & Steen, 2010).
Predictors of life satisfaction and happiness have oftentimes been found to differ from normally expected values. For example, no particular time in an individual’s life is exceptionally happier or unhappier than any other (Mogilner, Kamvar, & Aaker, 2010). Additionally, confrontation with crisis times is also not predictable by age (McCrae et al., 1999). With respect to gender, global well-being appears to be approximately equal in men and women cross culturally (Witter, Stock, Okun, & Haring, 1985). Additionally, race and ethnic group also have been found to be unrelated to psychological well-being and self-esteem (LaVeist, Thorpe, Pierre, Mance, & Williams, 2014; Schueller & Seligman, 2010; Witter et al., 1985). However, nationality has been found to be quite important in relation to subjective well-being levels, with individualistic cultures that are conducive to the expression of positive emotion and encouraging of the experience of happiness manifesting higher levels of subjective well-being and self-reported life satisfaction (Costa & Zonderman, 1987; Dolan, Peasgood, & White, 2008; Helliwell, 2003; Wrosch & Scheier, 2003). Finally, even though vast numbers of individuals see being well-off financially and materially as being intimately connected with happiness levels, studies have shown that once the basic necessities of life are provided for, the correlation between happiness and income is minimal (Angeles, 2011; Fujita & Diener, 2005; Kahneman, Krueger, Schkade, Schwarz, & Stone, 2006; Lyubomirsky, 2011; Verme, 2011; Wolbring, Keuschnigg, & Negele, 2013) with only a slight increase in happiness among high income earners (Zuzanek, 2013). It seems that though an absence of wealth may cause unhappiness and misery, its presence does not necessarily guarantee happiness (Diener, Horwitz, & Emmons, 1985; Judge, Piccolo, Podsakoff, Shaw, & Rich, 2010). Even drastic short-term increases or decreases in individual fortune seem to only temporarily modify levels of well-being due to hedonic adaptation (Diener & Scollon, 2014; Lyubomirsky, 2011; Mancini, Bonanno, & Clark, 2011; Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, 2006). Therefore, the idea that subjective well-being is inextricably tied to income levels and economic growth appears to be an illusion. Happiness seems to be available to all individuals irrespective of race, gender, age, and most income levels.
Some of the traits associated with happy individuals are optimism, self-esteem, personal control, and extraversion (Diener, 2013; Janoff-Bulman, 1989; Lyubomirsky, King, & Diener, 2005; Lyubomirsky, Sheldon, & Schkade, 2005; Meevissen, Peters, & Alberts, 2011; Peterson, Park, & Seligman, 2005; Rashid, 2014; Taylor & Brown, 1988). Additionally, happy individuals seem to manifest a personal bias, viewing themselves as more intelligent, ethical, healthier, and less prejudiced than others. Individuals who feel empowered tend to better manage stress, achieve more academically, and experience more happiness than those who are deprived of control (Judge & Larsen, 2001; Larsen & Ketelaar, 1991; Ochsner & Gross, 2005; Rashid, 2014; Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, 2006). An optimistic attitude wherein individuals sense an expectation for success is correlated with healthier and happier lives than is pessimism (Dember & Brooks, 1989; Judge & Larsen, 2001; Littman-Ovadia & Nir, 2014). Extraverts seem happier than introverts whether they are alone or with others, whether their occupations are solitary or social, whether their living circumstances are metropolitan or rural, or whether they live with others or alone (Diener, Inglehart, & Tay, 2012; Diener, Diener, Oishi, & Lucas, 2003; Emmons, 1991; Judge & Larsen, 2001; Layous & Lyubomirsky, 2012; Lucas & Baird, 2004; Norris, Larsen, Crawford, & Cacioppo, 2011; Specht, Egloff, & Schmukle, 2011; Tkach & Lyubomirsky, 2006). It is probably the case that extroversion causes increased subjective well-being, and increased subjective well-being causes increased outgoing behavior. Therefore, although close social relationships may certainly increase stress, it seems that the benefits outweigh the costs. Individuals with numerous intimate friendships tend to be healthier, enjoy higher positive affect, and live longer than those lacking such relationships (Friedrich, Cohen, & Wilturner, 1988; House, Umberson, & Landis, 1988; Lyubomirsky, 2011). It may be that the rampant levels of depression in modern culture are directly connected to impoverished social relationships and social bonds. For example, unsuccessful marital relationships are correlated with unhappiness, while close supportive marital relationships are viewed as joyous (Birditt, Hope, Brown, & Orbuch, 2012; DeMaris, Sanchez, & Krivickas, 2012; Dush & Amato, 2005; Lee & Bulanda, 2005; Lee, Seccombe, & Shehan, 1991). Of course, it may be that happy individuals are more prone to enter constructive marital relationships to begin with.
Work satisfaction is directly connected to subjective well-being and life satisfaction (Cozijnsen, Stevens, & Tilburg, 2010; Elton & Smart, 1988; Erdogan, Bauer, Truxillo, & Mansfield, 2012; Lyubomirsky, King, et al., 2005). This may be due to the fact that individuals tend to define their personal identity in terms of what they do, and through this realize a broader sense of community and network of constructive relationships, and thereby, a sense of belonging may be created through work, providing focus and meaning. Conversely, work may be unsatisfying if the related stresses are overwhelming, or when individuals are disengaged and bored. The ideal may be when the demands of the job match the skill set of an individual producing an optimal state of engagement or flow in which one is unselfconsciously absorbed in an activity with a focused mind, thereby producing transcendence through concentration or flow (Carpentier, Mageau, & Vallerand, 2011; Csikszentmihalyi, 1990, 1996, 1997, 2014a, 2014b; Salanova, Rodríguez-Sánchez, Schaufeli, & Cifre, 2013). From this perspective, happiness results from intense engagement in consciously challenging meaningful activities.
Finally, research has shown that an intimate connection exists between mental health and religion, possibly due to the fact that religious individuals are far less inclined to drug and alcohol abuse, divorce, unhappy marriages, and suicide (Calhoun, Cann, Tedeschi, & McMillan, 2000; Terrizzi, Shook, & Ventis, 2012; Messay, Dixon, & Rye, 2012; Myers, Eid, & Larsen, 2008; Shariff & Norenzayan, 2007). Consequently, religious individuals tend to be physically healthier and enjoy life due to healthier lifestyles (Daaleman, Cobb, & Frey, 2001; McIntosh, Poulin, Silver, & Holman, 2011; Newton & McIntosh, 2010). The relationship between religion and happiness transcends culture as those who are spiritually committed are far more likely to report high levels of life satisfaction directly related to the level of spiritual commitment (McCullough, Hoyt, Larson, Koenig, & Thoresen, 2000; Witter et al., 1985). Additionally, faith appears to assist individuals in coping with life crises (Ellison, Fang, Flannelly, & Steckler, 2013; George, Ellison, & Larson, 2002; Harvey, Barnes, & Greenwood, 1987; McGloshen & O’Bryant, 1988; Milevsky & Levitt, 2004; Momtaz, Ibrahim, Hamid, & Yahaya, 2010). In summary, there are clearly positive correlations between faith and well-being which may be related to numerous factors such as supportive relationships, the sense of meaning and purpose derived from spiritual practice, religious optimism, and finally a deep sense of hope in the face of the inevitability of death.
The term happiness, though popularly used, is somewhat abstract and difficult to define as it may refer to the positive affect at a given moment or a broad-based evaluation of life satisfaction and subjective well-being. Additionally, subjective well-being is a broad-based term used to evaluate the level of well-being of an individual. Such evaluations can be both negative and positive and subsume feelings and judgments about life satisfaction, levels of engagement and interest, and multitudinous variations of sadness and joy in relation to recreation, relationships, work, health, meaning, and purpose. Even though an evaluation of well-being may be subjective, it can be objectively measured by studying verbal and nonverbal behavior, attention, memory, facial measures, physiological responses, and general activity. A plethora of assessments of well-being exist, including: (a) Self-Anchoring Striving Scale (Angner, 2011; Tay & Diener, 2011), (b) Sixty-Second Happiness Measure (Fordyce, 1988; Verme, 2011), (c) PANAS ((Bargh & Ferguson, 2000; Bargh, 2011; McKenna, 2012; Morf & Horvath, 2010; Reid, 2011)Merz & Roesch, 2011; Shafir, Taylor, Atkinson, Langenecker, & Zubieta, 2013; Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, 2006), and the assessments used in this dissertation: (a) Subjective Happiness Scale (Lyubomirsky, Boehm, Kasri, & Zehm, 2011; Lyubomirsky, Dickerhoof, et al., 2011; Lyubomirsky & Tucker, 1998; Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, 2006; Tkach & Lyubomirsky, 2006), (b) Personal Growth Initiative Scale (Robitschek & Ashton, 2012; Robitschek et al., 2012), (c) Meaning in Life Questionnaire (Diener, Inglehart, et al., 2012; Pavot, 2013; Steger & Kashdan, 2008; Steger, Frazier, Oishi, & Kaler, 2006), (d) Satisfaction with Life Scale ADDIN CSL_CITATION { “citationItems” : [ { “id” : “ITEM-1”, “itemData” : { “DOI” : “10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143052”, “ISSN” : “1548-5951”, “PMID” : “22035241”, “abstract” : “Clinical anxiety disorders and elevated levels of anxiety vulnerability are characterized by cognitive biases, and this processing selectivity has been implicated in theoretical accounts of these conditions. We review research that has sought to evaluate the causal contributions such biases make to anxiety dysfunction and to therapeutically alleviate anxiety using cognitive-bias modification (CBM) procedures. After considering the purpose and nature of CBM methodologies, we show that variants designed to modify selective attention (CBM-A) or interpretation (CBM-I) have proven capable of reducing anxiety vulnerability and ameliorating dysfunctional anxiety. In addition to supporting the causal role of cognitive bias in anxiety vulnerability and dysfunction and illuminating the mechanisms that underpin such bias, the findings suggest that CBM procedures may have therapeutic promise within clinical settings. We discuss key issues within this burgeoning field of research and suggest future directions CBM research should take to maximize its theoretical and applied value.”, “author” : [ { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “MacLeod”, “given” : “Colin”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Mathews”, “given” : “Andrew”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” } ], “container-title” : “Annual Review of Clinical Psychology”, “id” : “ITEM-1”, “issue” : “1”, “issued” : { “date-parts” : [ [ “2012”, “1” ] ] }, “note” : “\n From Duplicate 2 ( \n \n Cognitive bias modification approaches to anxiety\n \n – MacLeod, C; Mathews, A )\n\n \n \n\n \n\n \n\n “, “page” : “189-217”, “title” : “Cognitive bias modification approaches to anxiety.”, “type” : “article-journal”, “volume” : “8” }, “uris” : [ “http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=f7e267bf-7189-4a0f-9943-254aff1bee9f” ] }, { “id” : “ITEM-2”, “itemData” : { “DOI” : “10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.12.006”, “ISSN” : “1873-7943”, “PMID” : “21349242”, “abstract” : “It is well established that anxious individuals show biases in information processing, such that they attend preferentially to threatening stimuli and interpret emotional ambiguity in a threatening way. It has also been established that these biases in attention and interpretation can causally influence anxiety. Recent advances in experimental work have involved the development of a paradigm known as Cognitive Bias Modification (CBM), a constellation of procedures which directly modify bias using computerised tasks. Excitingly, these procedures have been shown to reduce bias in attention to threat (CBM-A), and to promote a positive interpretive bias (CBM-I) in anxious populations; furthermore, these modifications are associated with reductions in anxiety. We believe that these techniques have the potential to create a real clinical impact for people with anxiety. Initial studies involved volunteer participants who reached criteria for clinical diagnoses to be made, but emerging evidence suggests that patients referred for therapy also benefit. For the purposes of experimentation researchers have normally looked at one procedure at a time. In order to try to maximise the potential clinical impact we wished to investigate whether the combination of the procedures would be more effective than either alone. We also wished to investigate whether the procedures could be carried out in routine clinical settings with patients referred to an out-patient psychological treatment service. We therefore carried out a pilot study using a combined approach of CBM-A and CBM-I with a sample of 13 anxious patients referred to an out-patient psychology service for cognitive therapy. The results showed successful reductions in threat related attentional and interpretive bias, as well as reductions in trait and state anxiety. Participant reports describe the procedures as acceptable, with the attentional task experienced as boring, but the interpretive one experienced as helpful. While recognising the methodological problems of the pilot study we believe that these results give indications that the techniques could provide an effective intervention for anxiety, and that further study is well justified.”, “author” : [ { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Brosan”, “given” : “Lee”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Hoppitt”, “given” : “Laura”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Shelfer”, “given” : “Lorna”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Sillence”, “given” : “Alison”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Mackintosh”, “given” : “Bundy”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” } ], “container-title” : “Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry”, “id” : “ITEM-2”, “issue” : “3”, “issued” : { “date-parts” : [ [ “2011”, “9” ] ] }, “page” : “258-64”, “title” : “Cognitive bias modification for attention and interpretation reduces trait and state anxiety in anxious patients referred to an out-patient service: Results from a pilot”, “type” : “article-journal”, “volume” : “42” }, “uris” : [ “http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=ac922437-f4f9-4bbd-be11-bb0eeec22cde” ] }, { “id” : “ITEM-3”, “itemData” : { “DOI” : “10.1586/ern.10.194”, “ISSN” : “1744-8360”, “PMID” : “21306216”, “abstract” : “Cognitive bias modification (CBM) is an innovative approach to modifying cognitive biases that confer vulnerability to anxiety. CBM interventions are designed to directly modify attention and interpretation biases via repeated practice on cognitive tasks. Analogue studies have demonstrated that CBM affects cognitive biases and anxiety in a number of anxiety conditions. Multisession CBM treatments have shown preliminary efficacy for generalized social phobia and generalized anxiety disorder, with effect sizes comparable to existing treatments. However, with any newly developing field, there are a number of important limitations of the existing data that need to be addressed before making firm conclusions regarding CBM’s efficacy for anxiety disorders. This article focuses on the theoretical rationale for CBM and the current evidence from analogue and clinical samples.”, “author” : [ { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Beard”, “given” : “Courtney”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” } ], “container-title” : “Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics”, “id” : “ITEM-3”, “issue” : “2”, “issued” : { “date-parts” : [ [ “2011”, “2” ] ] }, “page” : “299-311”, “title” : “Cognitive bias modification for anxiety: current evidence and future directions”, “type” : “article-journal”, “volume” : “11” }, “uris” : [ “http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=039a5f50-8576-4472-85b2-562250c145ba” ] }, { “id” : “ITEM-4”, “itemData” : { “DOI” : “10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02286.x”, “ISSN” : “1469-7610”, “PMID” : “20629805”, “abstract” : “BACKGROUND: Negative biases in the interpretation of ambiguous material have been linked to anxiety and mood problems. Accumulating data from adults show that positive and negative interpretation styles can be induced through cognitive bias modification (CBM) paradigms with accompanying changes in mood. Despite the therapeutic potential of positive training effects, training paradigms have not yet been explored in adolescents.\n\nMETHODS: Eighty-two healthy adolescents (aged 13-17 years) were randomly allocated to either positive or negative CBM training. To assess training effects on interpretation bias, participants read ambiguous situations followed by test sentences with positive or negative interpretations of the situation. Participants rated the similarity of these sentences to the previously viewed ambiguous situations. Training effects on negative and positive affect were assessed using visual analogue scales before and after training.\n\nRESULTS: After training, adolescents in the negative condition drew more negative and fewer positive interpretations of new ambiguous situations than adolescents in the positive condition. Within the positive condition, adolescents endorsed more positive than negative interpretations. In terms of mood changes, positive training resulted in a significant decrease in negative affect across participants, while the negative condition led to a significant decrease in positive affect among male participants only.\n\nCONCLUSION: This is the first study to demonstrate the plasticity of interpretation bias in adolescents. The immediate training effects on mood suggest that it may be possible to train a more positive interpretation style in youth, potentially helping to protect against anxiety and depressive symptoms.”, “author” : [ { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Lothmann”, “given” : “Claudia”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Holmes”, “given” : “Emily A EA”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Chan”, “given” : “Stella W Y”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Lau”, “given” : “Jennifer Y F”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” } ], “container-title” : “Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines”, “id” : “ITEM-4”, “issue” : “1”, “issued” : { “date-parts” : [ [ “2011”, “1” ] ] }, “note” : “\n From Duplicate 2 ( \n \n Cognitive bias modification training in adolescents: effects on interpretation biases and mood.\n \n – Lothmann, Claudia; Holmes, Emily A; Chan, Stella W Y; Lau, Jennifer Y F )\n\n \n \n\n \n\n \n\n “, “page” : “24-32”, “title” : “Cognitive bias modification training in adolescents: effects on interpretation biases and mood”, “type” : “article-journal”, “volume” : “52” }, “uris” : [ “http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=2e5aeec3-5649-4ad6-b6c8-6633330693b6” ] }, { “id” : “ITEM-5”, “itemData” : { “DOI” : “10.1037/a0023808”, “ISSN” : “1939-2117”, “PMID” : “21707134”, “abstract” : “OBJECTIVE: At least 3 randomized, placebo-controlled, double-blind studies have supported the efficacy of computerized attention modification programs (AMPs) in reducing symptoms of anxiety in patients diagnosed with an anxiety disorder. In this study we examined patient characteristics that predicted response to AMP in a large sample of individuals diagnosed with generalized social phobia.\n\nMETHOD: The sample comprised 112 individuals seeking treatment for generalized social phobia who completed a randomized clinical trial comparing AMP (n = 55) with a placebo condition (i.e., attention control condition; n = 57). We examined the following domains of baseline predictors of treatment response: (a) demographic characteristics (gender, age, ethnicity, years of education); (b) clinical characteristics (Axis I comorbidity, trait anxiety, depression); and (c) cognitive disturbance factors (attentional bias for social threat, social interpretation bias).\n\nRESULTS: Results revealed that ethnicity predicted treatment response across both conditions: Participants who self-identified as non-Caucasian displayed better overall response than did Caucasians. The only prescriptive variable to emerge was attentional bias for social threat at preassessment. Participants in the AMP group who exhibited larger attentional bias scores displayed significantly greater reductions in clinician-rated social anxiety symptoms than did their counterparts in the attention control condition.\n\nCONCLUSIONS: These results suggest that AMP may be targeted to individuals most likely to benefit from these programs.”, “author” : [ { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Amir”, “given” : “Nader”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Taylor”, “given” : “Charles T CT”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Donohue”, “given” : “Michael C MC”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” } ], “container-title” : “Journal of consulting and clinical psychology”, “id” : “ITEM-5”, “issue” : “4”, “issued” : { “date-parts” : [ [ “2011”, “8” ] ] }, “page” : “533-41”, “title” : “Predictors of response to an attention modification program in generalized social phobia.”, “type” : “article-journal”, “volume” : “79” }, “uris” : [ “http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=2e060169-7c41-4eb8-a32e-29964a86720f” ] }, { “id” : “ITEM-6”, “itemData” : { “DOI” : “10.1007/s10608-012-9445-8”, “ISSN” : “0147-5916”, “author” : [ { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Mobini”, “given” : “Sirous”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Reynolds”, “given” : “Shirley”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Mackintosh”, “given” : “Bundy”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” } ], “container-title” : “Cognitive Therapy and Research”, “id” : “ITEM-6”, “issue” : “1”, “issued” : { “date-parts” : [ [ “2013”, “3”, “14” ] ] }, “note” : “\n From Duplicate 2 ( \n \n Clinical Implications of Cognitive Bias Modification for Interpretative Biases in Social Anxiety: An Integrative Literature Review\n \n – Mobini, Sirous; Reynolds, Shirley; Mackintosh, Bundy )\n\n \n \n\n \n\n \n\n “, “page” : “173-182”, “title” : “Clinical implications of cognitive bias modification for interpretative biases in social anxiety: An integrative literature review”, “type” : “article-journal”, “volume” : “37” }, “uris” : [ “http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=e69e3969-df53-4664-a8f1-08a40058413b” ] }, { “id” : “ITEM-7”, “itemData” : { “DOI” : “10.1016/j.beth.2009.01.002”, “ISSN” : “1878-1888”, “PMID” : “20171329”, “abstract” : “Training participants to select threat or nonthreat interpretations of emotionally ambiguous stimuli or passively exposing them to valenced scenarios can modify later interpretation of ambiguity. However, only when encouraged to actively select meanings do congruent changes in emotional response occur during training itself (Mathews & Mackintosh, 2000). The present study assessed the more critical question of whether active training is also necessary for modifying subsequent emotional responses to images of new ambiguous scenarios presented after training. As predicted, active training did lead participants to rate their images of emotionally ambiguous scenarios as being more unpleasant after training as compared to a matched passive condition. This finding supports the view that active generation of meaning during interpretive training is critical for the modification of later emotional responses.”, “author” : [ { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Hoppitt”, “given” : “Laura”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Mathews”, “given” : “Andrew”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Yiend”, “given” : “Jenny”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Mackintosh”, “given” : “Bundy”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” } ], “container-title” : “Behavior Therapy”, “id” : “ITEM-7”, “issue” : “1”, “issued” : { “date-parts” : [ [ “2010”, “3” ] ] }, “note” : “\n From Duplicate 1 ( \n \n Cognitive bias modification: The critical role of active training in modifying emotional responses\n \n – Hoppitt, Laura; Mathews, Andrew; Yiend, Jenny; Mackintosh, Bundy )\n\n \n \n\n \n\n \n\n From Duplicate 2 ( \n \n Cognitive bias modification: The critical role of active training in modifying emotional responses\n \n – Hoppitt, Laura; Mathews, Andrew; Yiend, Jenny; Mackintosh, Bundy )\n\n \n \n\n From Duplicate 2 ( \n \n \n Cognitive bias modification: The critical role of active training in modifying emotional responses\n \n \n – Hoppitt, L; Mathews, A; Yiend, J; Mackintosh, B )\n\n \n \n\n \n\n \n\n \n\n \n\n “, “page” : “73-81”, “title” : “Cognitive bias modification: The critical role of active training in modifying emotional responses”, “type” : “article-journal”, “volume” : “41” }, “uris” : [ “http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=a8d6a4a9-0dd9-4a80-8720-b06fc99f36ea” ] }, { “id” : “ITEM-8”, “itemData” : { “DOI” : “10.1037/a0024355”, “ISSN” : “1939-1455”, “PMID” : “21728399”, “abstract” : “Cognitive biases have been theorized to play a critical role in the onset and maintenance of anxiety and depression. Cognitive bias modification (CBM), an experimental paradigm that uses training to induce maladaptive or adaptive cognitive biases, was developed to test these causal models. Although CBM has generated considerable interest in the past decade, both as an experimental paradigm and as a form of treatment, there have been no quantitative reviews of the effect of CBM on anxiety and depression. This meta-analysis of 45 studies (2,591 participants) assessed the effect of CBM on cognitive biases and on anxiety and depression. CBM had a medium effect on biases (g = 0.49) that was stronger for interpretation (g = 0.81) than for attention (g = 0.29) biases. CBM further had a small effect on anxiety and depression (g = 0.13), although this effect was reliable only when symptoms were assessed after participants experienced a stressor (g = 0.23). When anxiety and depression were examined separately, CBM significantly modified anxiety but not depression. There was a nonsignificant trend toward a larger effect for studies including multiple training sessions. These findings are broadly consistent with cognitive theories of anxiety and depression that propose an interactive effect of cognitive biases and stressors on these symptoms. However, the small effect sizes observed here suggest that this effect may be more modest than previously believed.”, “author” : [ { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Hallion”, “given” : “LS Lauren S”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” }, { “dropping-particle” : “”, “family” : “Ruscio”, “given” : “Ayelet Meron AM”, “non-dropping-particle” : “”, “parse-names” : false, “suffix” : “” } ], “container-title” : “Psychological bulletin”, “id” : “ITEM-8”, “issue” : “6”, “issued” : { “date-parts” : [ [ “2011”, “11” ] ] }, “page” : “940-58”, “title” : “A meta-analysis of the effect of cognitive bias modification on anxiety and depression.”, “type” : “article-journal”, “volume” : “137” }, “uris” : [ “http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=54d9b939-d66e-47eb-80c6-9aab98e50870” ] } ], “mendeley” : { “formattedCitation” : “(Amir, Taylor, & Donohue, 2011; Beard, 2011; Brosan, Hoppitt, Shelfer, Sillence, & Mackintosh, 2011; Hallion & Ruscio, 2011; Hoppitt, Mathews, Yiend, & Mackintosh, 2010; Lothmann, Holmes, Chan, & Lau, 2011; MacLeod & Mathews, 2012; Mobini, Reynolds, & Mackintosh, 2013)”, “plainTextFormattedCitation” : “(Amir, Taylor, & Donohue, 2011; Beard, 2011; Brosan, Hoppitt, Shelfer, Sillence, & Mackintosh, 2011; Hallion & Ruscio, 2011; Hoppitt, Mathews, Yiend, & Mackintosh, 2010; Lothmann, Holmes, Chan, & Lau, 2011; MacLeod & Mathews, 2012; Mobini, Reynolds, & Mackintosh, 2013)”, “previouslyFormattedCitation” : “(Amir, Taylor, & Donohue, 2011; Beard, 2011; Brosan, Hoppitt, Shelfer, Sillence, & Mackintosh, 2011; Hallion & Ruscio, 2011; Hoppitt, Mathews, Yiend, & Mackintosh, 2010; Lothmann, Holmes, Chan, & Lau, 2011; MacLeod & Mathews, 2012; Mobini, Reynolds, & Mackintosh, 2013)” }, “properties” : { “noteIndex” : 0 }, “schema” : “https://github.com/citation-style-language/schema/raw/master/csl-citation.json” }, (e) The Gratitude Questionnaire (Lyubomirsky, Dickerhoof, et al., 2011; McCullough, Emmons, & Tsang, 2002).
From a psychotherapeutic perspective, the study of well-being has as its main goal the constructive transformation of individual lives, and measures of subjective well-being act as a key element in pursuit of this goal. Not surprisingly, a great deal of research indicates that there is a strong correlation between elevated levels of subjective well-being and effective functioning in areas such as work and income, health and longevity, and social relations (Diener, Fujita, Tay, & Biswas-Diener, 2012; Diener, 2012, 2013). Elevated subjective well-being has been found to be strongly correlated with high levels of sociality; it is most likely that causality between these variables is bidirectional in that individuals possessing a large number of friends and family manifest higher levels of subjective well-being, and individuals with high well-being generally have more productive social relationships than those with low levels of life satisfaction (Brannan, Biswas-Diener, Mohr, Mortazavi, & Stein, 2013; Diener, 2012, 2013). Clearly supportive relationships have a causal influence on high levels of subjective well-being, and studies have shown that people are generally happier around other people (Kahneman et al., 2006; Kahneman & Krueger, 2006). Additionally, married individuals manifest higher levels of subjective well-being than non-married individuals (Kahneman & Krueger, 2006).
Other studies have shown that individuals who start with high subjective levels of well-being generally possess higher levels of social ability, self-confidence, warmth, and leadership ability, and tend to create constructive social support systems (Ashkanasy & Humphrey, 2011; Brown, Diekman, Tennial, & Solomon, 2011; Cunningham, 1988; Isen, Daubman, & Nowicki, 1987). Individuals possessing high life satisfaction before marriage are more likely to become married and sustain happy marriages (Lyubomirsky & Layous, 2013; Oishi, Diener, & Lucas, 2007). Those with high subjective well-being tend to earn more money irrespective of occupation, and are more likely to be satisfied with work (Diener, Nickerson, Lucas, & Sandvik, 2002; Sutin, Costa, Miech, & Eaton, 2009; Zhong & Mitchell, 2012). In other words, individuals with high levels of well-being create career success and economic success, have higher ratings from superiors, are seen as more productive, creative, and dependable and are generally seen as excellent organizational citizens (Staw, Sutton, & Pelled, 1994). Health and longevity are also improved by subjective well-being. Individuals with high subjective well-being report less unpleasant physical symptoms, better overall health (Røysamb, Tambs, Reichborn-Kjennerud, Neale, & Harris, 2003), are more resistant to viruses, have stronger immune systems and healthier cardiovascular systems, have fewer lifestyle diseases, and enjoy longer lives (Diener & Chan, 2011; Røysamb et al., 2003).
Individuals who pursue high subjective levels of well-being engage more often in altruistic activities (Lum & Lightfoot, 2005; Thomas, Gray, & McGinty, 2012), are more cooperative (Bogaert, Boone, & Declerck, 2008; Salanova et al., 2013; Wilt et al., 2012), have higher levels of confidence in government, and have lower levels of intolerance for racial groups and immigrants (Diener & Tov, 2007; Wolbring, 2013). Consequently, the well-being of individuals derives from a healthy society, and highly subjective levels of well-being contribute toward stable and productive societies. But even though high levels of well-being generally cause beneficial functioning at both individual and societal levels, if reported levels are too extreme they may be detrimental to achievement in specific areas (Oishi et al., 2007). It may be that an inordinate striving for ecstatic levels of well-being may be ironically detrimental if continuous euphoria is seen to be a desirous goal.
Well-being has been studied in various psychological frameworks based on biological theories analyzing genetic predispositions (Archontaki, Lewis, & Bates, 2013; Diener, 1996; Franz et al., 2012; Fujita & Diener, 2005; Nistor, 2011; Røysamb et al., 2003), telic theories relating to goal achievement (Brunstein, 1993; Lyubomirsky, Dickerhoof, et al., 2011; Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, 2006), need theories (Ryff & Singer, 1996; Tay & Diener, 2011), and Self-Determination Theory (Carpentier et al., 2011; Gagné & Deci, 2005; Kasser, Rosenblum, & Sameroff, 2014; Lyubomirsky, 2011; Ryan & Deci, 2000; Sheldon, 2014). Also, both top-down theories (which maintain that a positive state of mind induces constructive experience and interpretation) and bottom-up theories (which posit that negative and positive moments are added together to produce perceived subjective well-being) have been employed to analyze well-being (Lyubomirsky & Tucker, 1998; Røysamb et al., 2003; Schimmack, Krause, Wagner, & Schupp, 2009). Related to these diametrically opposed perspectives, it is an open question as to whether well-being is a trait inclining individuals to react positively or a state which simply manifests due to the accumulation of constructive experiences.
Many cognitive theories investigating processes such as attention, interpretation, and memory suggest that individuals with high levels of subjective well-being have a tendency to focus on positive stimuli, interpret events in a positive light, and remember events with a positive memory bias (Hoshino, 1991; Verde, Stone, Hatch, & Schnall, 2010). Additionally, the tendency to direct attention toward others seems to be a significant predictor of well-being. Individuals who ruminate are inclined to worry and report lower subjective well-being (Kross, Duckworth, Ayduk, Tsukayama, & Mischel, 2011; Tkach & Lyubomirsky, 2006; Tran, et al., 2011). Focusing attention inward may cause individuals to experience significantly lower levels of well-being. Individuals with high subjective well-being generally interpret neutral or ambiguous events positively. Dispositionally happy individuals may have no difference in the amount of negative and positive events in their lives, and simply may tend to remember events in a positive way by way of a protective memory bias.
Some theories of the causes of well-being are evolutionary, and among other things, imply that feelings of well-being assist in survival (Huang & Bargh, 2014; Toates, 2006). The evolutionary survival value of negative emotions such as anxiety, fear, and anger are clear, however, well-being also provides adaptive advantages. For example, a “broaden and build theory” (Fredrickson, 2001; Fredrickson, 2004; Garland et al., 2010; Tugade, Fredrickson, & Barrett, 2004; Tugade & Fredrickson, 2004) posits that positive emotions permit individuals to expand cognitive and behavior repertoires thereby constructively impacting psychological, social, and intellectual abilities over time. The implication is that elevated subjective well-being and constructive affect produce a condition in which individuals may explore environments and goals. From this perspective, well-being clearly contributes adaptive advantages which facilitate evolutionary success and survival.
Social Comparison and Well-Being
From the perspective of social comparison theory, individuals generally experience higher levels of well-being if they perceive that they are better off than others (Diener, Inglehart, et al., 2012; Lyubomirsky & Lepper, 1999; Lyubomirsky, Boehm, et al., 2011; Neff, 2003; Nolen-Hoeksema, 2012; Posten et al., 2014; Wood, 1989). In fact, some studies indicate that such relative standards derived from social comparison are the most important factor in predicting personal satisfaction (Diener, Larsen, & Emmons, 1984; Graham, Bradshaw, Surood, & Kline, 2014). Additionally, proponents of adaptation theory (Abbey, 1987; Diener, Lucas, & Scollon, 2006; Diener et al., 2003; Mancini et al., 2011; Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, 2012; Tugade et al., 2004; Waterman, 2007) hold that individuals use their own past as a standard for comparison. In other words, if current life conditions are perceived to be better than those of the past, higher levels of well-being occur. However, theories of hedonic adaptation propose that the potency of the influence of past events on current levels of happiness decreases over time, and current events tend to become the new standard of comparison in the creation of feelings of well-being, giving rise to the ‘hedonic treadmill’ effect (Fujita & Diener, 2005; Lyubomirsky, 2011; Mancini et al., 2011; Waterman, 2007) in which more recent events only temporarily increase subjective well-being to become the new standard for self-comparison. Hence, individual happiness ‘set points’ change over time and are impacted by both positive and negative life-altering events.
The malleability of set points due to external influences is analogous to the epigenetic mapping between genes and the transmission of information in cellular metabolism that is modifiable within the context of environmental interactions (Huang & Bargh, 2014; Lipton, 2008). According to this theory, genes are not able to turn themselves on and off; for this they need external influences. These epigenetic factors are consciously controllable and can substantially modify genes. Gene switches are comparable to computer programs and changing beliefs modifies the instructions. Genes can be switched into on states and off states by feelings, emotions, and thoughts. These modifications take place in the cell membrane that responds to the energy of belief. The cell membrane possesses chemical receptors as well as energy receptors that are sensitive to emotions and thoughts. The cell membrane acts intelligently through channeling proteins and switching genes permitting environment influences on proteins that are vital for the activation of DNA. The programming language that cell walls understand is the language of energy and emotions. Emotional choices create energy patterns and blockages that are the environment in which the cell develops. Through epigenetics, the environmental programming given to cells modulates genetic activity. Therefore, belief transmitted chemically and energetically through the cell wall and switching DNA is a key to self-healing and transformation. The epigenetic explanation demonstrates much greater control than that implied by biological determinism; modification of emotions, thoughts, and behaviors is the key to transformation at both the molecular level and macro level of the whole organism.
Numerous studies have considered connections between macro levels of well-being and the relationship to numerous factors such as gender, education levels, age, religion, marriage, divorce, social relationships in general, income, and culture. With respect to gender, well-being studies have indicated that men and women exhibit similar levels of subjective well-being (Fujita, Diener, & Sandvik, 1991; Huang & Bargh, 2014; Nolen-Hoeksema, 2012; Sherman et al., 2010). Numerous studies show a slight correlation between education level and well-being, but the link connecting intelligence and subjective well-being is minimal, while emotional intelligence is highly correlated (Brett et al., 2012; Wojcik & Ditto, 2014). Although youth has been consistently correlated with well-being (Angner, 2011; Brett et al., 2012; Cameron, 1975), numerous studies have shown that life satisfaction oftentimes increases with age (Hyde, Maher, & Elavsky, 2013; Warr, Butcher, & Robertson, 2004; Zhang & Leung, 2002). Generally speaking, life satisfaction has been found to increase among individuals from ages 40 to 65, with both negative and positive affect minimally decreasing into old age with substantial declines in well-being only occurring near death (Hyde et al., 2013; Warr et al., 2004; Zhang & Leung, 2002). Cross culturally, the relationship between subjective well-being and age is consistent; however, life satisfaction declines substantially in economically challenged countries (Blanchflower & Oswald, 2011; Swift et al., 2014). Studies have shown that religiously and spiritually oriented individuals manifest higher levels of well-being based on religious participation, strength of affiliation, prayer, and relationship with God (Dierendonck & Mohan, 2006; Messay et al., 2012; Newton & McIntosh, 2010; Rosmarin, Pirutinsky, Cohen, Galler, & Krumrei, 2011; Sillick & Cathcart, 2014; Wilmoth, Adams-Price, Turner, Blaney, & Downey, 2014; Witter et al., 1985). It is possible that the linkage is due to an enhanced sense of purpose and meaning, social networks, and support systems.
As individuals are generally happier in groups and while experiencing social interactions, the quality and number of social relationships appears to be causative of high levels of subjective well-being (Caunt et al., 2013; Diener & Chan, 2011; Diener & Scollon, 2014; Mogilner et al., 2010; Ng & Diener, 2014). In groups, individuals manifest more positive affect, whereas loneliness is highly correlated with depression (Dolphin & Hennessy, 2014; Masi et al., 2010). Additionally, social interactions tend to buffer major life stressors such as unemployment and bereavement (Lyubomirsky, 2011; Mancini et al., 2011; McKee-Ryan, Song, Wanberg, & Kinicki, 2005; Myers & Diener, 1995). Subjective well-being levels have been found to be higher with married individuals, however, due to hedonic adaptation, individuals tend to return to baseline happiness set points after marriage (Bao, 2012; Uglanova & Staudinger, 2013). As a concomitant to this phenomena, divorced individuals show lower levels of average well-being, and divorce has shown to cause substantial declines in subjective well-being without the rebound effect to prior levels of well-being (Lucas, 2005, 2007). Again, it may be the case that many individuals who divorce had lower levels of well-being prior to marriage. Such correlations with respect to marriage and divorce and well-being have been shown to be consistent across cultures (Lee & Ono, 2012; Nes, Røysamb, Harris, Czajkowski, & Tambs, 2010).
The relationship between unemployment, income and well-being patterns has been studied with unemployment being highly correlated with negative impact on subjective well-being, with individuals generally not returning to happiness baseline set points even after gaining employment (Diener, Inglehart, et al., 2012; Kahneman & Krueger, 2006; Lyubomirsky, 2011; Oishi et al., 2007; Verme, 2011). Therefore, it seems that unemployment may even impact long-term happiness. However, the relationship between income level and well-being is positively correlated, but yields diminishing returns as increased income levels substantially increase well-being for those at poverty levels or in economically undeveloped nations, but wealth and well-being correlations decrease, demonstrating a declining marginal utility (Naudé, Amorós, & Cristi, 2014). Also, individuals who view money and material possessions as primary interests tend to manifest low levels of satisfaction and positive affect (Kasser et al., 2014; Norris, Lambert, DeWall, & Fincham, 2012; Stillman, Fincham, Vohs, Lambert, & Phillips, 2012). Cross culturally, life satisfaction has been highly correlated with national income level (Wolbring et al., 2013), and yet nationally oriented social and psychological factors are more correlated with emotional well-being. Hence, though income level impacts emotional well-being, numerous other factors are even more impactful. More precisely, the effects of culture in industrialized societies and associated economic development are correlated with high levels of subjective well-being (Davidson, Pacek, & Radcliff, 2013; Tay & Diener, 2011). However, in many cultures poverty is not found to be devastating of life satisfaction (Biswas-Diener & Patterson, 2010; Diener, Oishi, & Ryan, 2013). For example, low income individuals in the United States have lower levels of life satisfaction and higher levels of negative affect than many individuals living in even more dire circumstances in Calcutta (Diener, Ng, Harter, & Arora, 2010; Diener et al., 2003; Tay & Diener, 2011). It seems that other factors such as human rights, democratic rights, and longevity may be explanatory of varying levels of subjective well-being.
Along with economic indicators such as the GDP and social indicators such as literacy rates or crime, more comprehensive measures of quality of life which monitor well-being may be useful (Brett et al., 2012; Carpentier et al., 2011; Diener & Diener, 2011; Diener, Inglehart, et al., 2012; Diener et al., 2003; Emmons & Stern, 2013; Forgeard, Jayawickreme, Kern, & Seligman, 2011; Gámez, Chmielewski, Kotov, Ruggero, & Watson, 2011; Jordan, Masters, Hooker, Ruiz, & Smith, 2013; Pavot, 2013). This would reflect the fact that many individuals see happiness and well-being as more important than merely materialistic goals; this would also be applicable in modern societies where individuals have the ability to seek more a fulfilling life than that provided by economic subsistence alone. Therefore, new measures of well-being are necessary to provide a more comprehensive picture of the functioning of well-being in modern societies and varieties of cultural contexts where individuals experience varying levels of subjective well-being and psychological needs. Thus, research such as the present study, with a view to examining the efficacy of psychotherapeutic interventions and the nexus with subjective well-being are significant.
Mindfulness Based Cognitive Therapy
MBCT utilizes elements derived from cognitive behavioral therapy and ancient Buddhist mindfulness practices to treat a wide range of psychological conditions including depression (Batink, Peeters, Geschwind, van Os, & Wichers, 2013), anxiety disorders (King et al., 2013; McManus, Muse, Surawy, Hackmann, & Williams, 2014; Surawy, McManus, Muse, & Williams, 2014), bipolar disorders (Cotton et al., 2015; Williams et al., 2008), pain management (Moore & Martin, 2014), chronic unhappiness (Collard, Avny, & Boniwell, 2008; Mason & Hargreaves, 2001), and insomnia (Ong, Ulmer, & Manber, 2012; Yook, Lee, Ryu, & Kim, 2008). Essentially the therapeutic goal of MBCT is the incorporation and realization of a radically new perspective and mode of being in relation to feelings and thoughts rather than addressing specific cognitive difficulties. The mindfulness construct itself has been defined as follows: Mindfulness is the awareness that emerges through paying attention on purpose, in the present moment, and nonjudgmentally to things as they are (Kabat-Zinn, 2003).
Numerous practices exist aimed at developing mindfulness such as meditation, walking meditation, breathing exercises, body scanning, and increased awareness of everyday activities. The goal of these methods is the promotion of augmented awareness of individual thoughts and feelings rather than pronounced effort at modification of specific thought content. As such, MBCT integrates elements of mindfulness and cognitive therapy, but with a modified goal orientation (Baer, Smith, & Hopkins, 2006; Collard et al., 2008; Kabat-Zinn, 2003; McManus et al., 2014). The emphasis is laid upon acceptance of thoughts and emotions, present moment awareness, and self-compassion rather than engaging in attempts to fight or avoid unwanted feelings and thoughts, which oftentimes intensifies psychological difficulties. Hence, in this sense MBCT is radically different from traditional CBT in that the main focus is simply placed upon awareness of feelings and thoughts and concomitant metacognitive awareness rather than alteration of thought content. Although this fundamental difference in perspective exists, the resulting diminution of destructive thought patterns occurs via either approach. For example, from the viewpoint of MBCT, a thought such as “I am no good” may be seen as simply another impermanent cognition that rises and sets during meditation, whereas the traditional approach of CBT advocates challenging the rational validity of the statement. Methodologically, mindfulness permits the flow of thoughts and feelings without effort at transforming or avoiding rather than challenging dysfunctional beliefs and creating new interpretations. Hence, MBCT takes an intuitive thought process focused perspective rather than a thought content focused one.
Summarizing, the underlying mechanism in MBCT transformation is founded upon the enhancement of metacognitive awareness through the realization of an altered mode of being wherein an individual is able to become aware of the discrepancy between a subjective viewpoint of how things ‘should be’ versus how things ‘are in reality’. Of course such an awareness can clearly become a source of increased suffering and dissatisfaction unless the focus of attention is integrated with acceptance of the present moment reality. Through the cultivation of nonjudgmental current moment awareness along with acceptance, negative cycles of rumination about the past or fears of the future may be minimized by decreasing generalized cognitive reactivity. In this way individual attention may be intentionally allocated to more flexible behavioral and cognitive responses. By consciously labeling feelings and thoughts as mental events, stressing cognitions inevitably diminish and less demand is placed upon cognitive energy resources. Effectively, present moment awareness may be utilized to enhance psychological well-being and emotional balance by minimizing habitual reaction patterns and promoting conscious response to experiences. Instead of avoiding or suppressing experiences or cognitions through elaboration and becoming enmeshed in their content, mindfulness promotes the observation of these elements as transient events in the mind through non-identified detachment, the adoption of a nonjudgmental attitude toward both unpleasant and pleasant experiences and generally increased openness and acceptance. As such, advanced mindfulness is well correlated with life satisfaction, optimism, positive affect, self-esteem, and diminished rumination (Caprara, Steca, Alessandri, Abela, & McWhinnie, 2011; Collard et al., 2008; Grossman, 2010). Hence, the range of conditions to which MBCT is applicable is extensive, its connection to traditional CBT clear, and its efficacy is well established.
The Search for Meaning in Life and Well-Being
Numerous studies have confirmed a connection between well-being and meaning in life (Heintzelman & King, 2014; Heintzelman, Trent, & King, 2013; Hicks, Schlegel, & King, 2010; Hicks, Trent, Davis, & King, 2012; Kashdan et al., 2013; McIntosh et al., 2011; Schlegel, Hicks, King, & Arndt, 2011; Steger & Kashdan, 2008; Steger & Frazier, 2005; Stillman et al., 2012). The human desire for self-comprehension and understanding of the external world and universe is made manifest by cognitive and behavioral pursuits. It seems that through self-understanding and comprehending the universe at large, humans attempt to comprehend their place in life. However, there is a great deal of variance in the efforts from individual to individual relative to an active search for meaning, particularly in terms of the intensity of the effort applied to creating or augmenting the purpose of life. Since the search for meaning acts as a fundamental motivational force, it seems that it must necessarily be part of a healthy psychological existence wherein individuals pursue new opportunities and challenges based on the desire to comprehend and organize experiences.
Although some researchers have intimated that the search for meaning is indicative of frustrated needs (Flanagan, 2010; Steger & Kashdan, 2008) or conflicting motivations (Cozzolino & Blackie, 2013; Cranney, 2013; Ritchie, Sedikides, Wildschut, Arndt, & Gidron, 2011), it may be the case that the quest for meaning is rooted in numerous underlying motivations. Possibly, deficits in happiness may stimulate the search for meaning, and it may be dependent on individual cognitive styles related to understanding and organizing information and individual cognitive and intellectual preferences. The quest for meaning may also be related to the fact that some individuals simply question everything, or to the fact that they have a propensity for the novel and are more curious than others. It may also be related to the pursuit of future goals or the inability to relinquish negative past experiences.
Constructive developmental theories that focus on self-understanding and socialization emphasize the development of meaning and the process of meaning making longitudinally (Cook-Greuter & Soulen, 2007; Cook-Greuter, 2004; Ingersoll & Cook-Greuter, 2007). Consideration is inevitably given to individual interpretations of experience over time by elaborating the ongoing process of self-understanding, development, and concomitant meaning system evolution and psychosocial growth. This theory posits that individuals actively construct methods of understanding themselves in the world, that identifiable stages and patterns of consciousness develop in specific invariant sequences, that orders of consciousness are inevitably more complex through time, and finally that levels of consciousness dictate and influence subjective perception and awareness. From a horizontal perspective, the search for meaning may be primarily related to personality traits such as openness to experience, and it is therefore a desirable psychological quality that compels individuals to search for meaning when they feel it is lacking. In other words, individuals may search for meaning due to being compelled to frustration in their personal growth and simply see the quest for meaning as a significant element in that quest.
It may also be the case that the search for meaning is directly associated with cognitive styles, particularly with the tendency to question authority and the status quo, and a negative viewpoint toward both the past and the present (Eastwood, Frischen, Fenske, & Smilek, 2012; Kim, Seto, Davis, & Hicks, 2014; Park, Park, & Peterson, 2010; Steger, Oishi, & Kesebir, 2011). From this viewpoint the search for meaning relates to opening up new avenues for life fulfillment, rumination on negative past experiences, and a feeling of futility toward current circumstances. In other words, both positive and negative motivations and orientations may be influential in spurring a search for meaning. It is therefore possible that those seeking meaning in life may not have a future time perspective, but are rather focused more on the past and present. Therefore, the search for meaning may not truly express a fundamental psychological need to comprehend existence, but may in fact represent psychological traits and correlation with numerous personality and cognitive variables resulting from psychological factors such as unhappiness about present circumstances and past experiences, over-thinking, and anxiety in addition to positive factors such as curiosity, receptivity, investigating, questioning, and absorption or flow.
The Pursuit of Happiness
Deeply embedded in our culture is the notion of the right or even duty of every individual to pursue happiness. In fact, it is an integral dimension in the lives of most individuals and even an obsession for some (Layous & Lyubomirsky, 2012; Lyubomirsky, Dickerhoof, et al., 2011; Oishi et al., 2007; Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, 2006; Zhong & Mitchell, 2012). From a research point of view, many attempts have been made to ascertain the value of individual strategies to promote self-reported happiness through an examination of the relationship between happiness strategies, personality traits, and subjective levels of well-being with (a) happiness associated with frequency of positive affective states of consciousness (Garland et al., 2010; Nickerson, Diener, & Schwarz, 2011; Nisbet, Zelenski, & Murphy, 2011); (b) judgments of happiness relating to generalized affective self-appraisals and inherently subjective affect ratings (Diener & Scollon, 2014); (c) long-term happiness represented by the experience of positive well-being over long periods of time (Caunt et al., 2013; Lyubomirsky, Dickerhoof, et al., 2011). Investigations of the causes of happiness involve demographics, life circumstances, intentional behaviors, and also personality traits (Caunt et al., 2013; Kaczmarek et al., 2013; Lyubomirsky & Layous, 2013). Research has found that ethnicity, age, and gender play a negligible role, explaining only minimally variances in happiness levels (Bailey & Snyder, 2010; Herian, Tay, Hamm, & Diener, 2014; Nolen-Hoeksema & Aldao, 2011). Some studies have indicated that biologically based, enduring dispositional personality traits account for a significant percentage of subjective well-being (Hampson, 2012; Kandler, Bleidorn, Riemann, Angleitner, & Spinath, 2011; Olesen, 2011).
Extraversion and neuroticism have been found to be strongly correlated with well-being and happiness with extraverts reporting higher levels than introverts and neurotic individuals reporting lower levels of well-being than those who are emotionally stable (Aghajani et al., 2013; Quevedo et al., 2011; Zhang & Tsingan, 2013). It is possible that extraversion permits individuals to be exposed to a greater level of mood-relevant information, and is involved in a causal sequence conducive to happiness through seeking out social behaviors and activities that increase positive moods and overall happiness synergistically, involving conscious self-regulatory actions and strategies protective of increased happiness levels. As extraverts are inevitably involved in a greater number of social situations, these may act as an inducement for constructive sociability and an inevitable engagement in volitional happiness increasing strategies. As intentional behavior is amenable to conscious modification, it may be key to prescriptive programs for increase in well-being such as CBT/REBT. In other words, individuals may engage in intentional activities to consciously promote long-term happiness. Of course, activities need to be selected properly to avoid those that either do nothing or are actually detrimental.
In summary, it seems that circumstantial factors are explanatory for only a minute portion of variation in happiness, while personality and dispositional traits are critical. Substantial portions of happiness variance may be influenced by intentional behaviors and strategies to control mood through self-regulatory mechanisms to control both transient mood and even affect long-term happiness (Labroo, Mukhopadhyay, & Dong, 2014; Livingstone & Srivastava, 2012; Warner & Vroman, 2011). Such mechanisms may include (a) social affiliation, which is strongly tied to happiness and a reduction of dysphoria (Querstret & Cropley, 2013) and is associated with increases in happiness through social activity (Layous, Nelson, & Lyubomirsky, 2013); (b) efforts to ward off unwanted thoughts that may unintentionally maintain or even exacerbate negative emotion (Gruber, Harvey, & Gross, 2012; McRae et al., 2012; Webb, Miles, & Sheeran, 2012); (c) an instrumental commitment to and realization of personal goals or goal pursuit associated with well-being and positive moods (Herrmann & Brandstätter, 2013; Sheldon, 2014); (d) active leisure, which has been associated with well-being possibly due to a redirection of focus away from the individual self (Engberg et al., 2012; Mobily, 2014) resulting in lower levels of anxiety, depression, and stress to improve mood and increase the probability of ‘flow’(Havitz, Kaczynski, & Mannell, 2013); (e) religion, which has been found to be a strong predictor of well-being (Hill et al., 2012; Wilmoth et al., 2014) possibly by providing increased social connectedness and life purpose as well as diminishing maladaptive behaviors (Neff & Pommier, 2013); (f) direct attempts at simply assuming an attitude of happiness through conscious acting out the expression of happiness (Chang, Zhang, Hitchman, Qiu, & Liu, 2014; Kemp & Kopp, 2011; Shafir et al., 2013).
The Art of Happiness
Subjective factors seem to be relatively important in explaining happiness (Owens & Derakshan, 2013; Owens, Koster, & Derakshan, 2012). It seems that individuals actively filter and respond to life events, and this subjective process may explain the differential influence of objective factors. Therefore, analyzing the behaviors, goals, and cognitive differences between exceptionally happy individuals through an examination of the motivational, behavioral, and cognitive processes through which these individuals create and sustain happiness through deriving satisfaction from their experiences forms the basis of a happiness art of living. The ways in which happy people evaluate and respond differently than less happy individuals has been researched and measured, and provides useful insights (Diener, Inglehart, et al., 2012; Holmes & Mathews, 2010; Lyubomirsky & Ross, 1997; Lyubomirsky & Tucker, 1998; Schwartz et al., 2002). Since happiness seems to be a subjective phenomena, it seems appropriate that information gathered relative to happiness levels is best derived from the subjective evaluation of individuals under consideration through such assessments as those used in this dissertation: (a) Subjective Happiness Scale, (b) Personal Growth Initiative Scale, (c) Meaning in Life Questionnaire, (d) Satisfaction with Life Scale, and (e) Gratitude Questionnaire.
It seems that happiness represents not merely a transient construct related to mood states, but rather a more fundamental enduring phenomena (Caunt et al., 2013; Kaczmarek et al., 2013; Lyubomirsky & Layous, 2013). It may be that happy individuals respond to events and utilize distinctly different decision-making strategies manifesting consistent patterns of content with their options and decisions. In relation to CBT/REBT, perfectionistic thinking may relate to finding the absolute optimal choice vs. simply finding an acceptable option. In fact, the perfectionistic decision-making strategy is actually negatively correlated with happiness (Brockmann & Yan, 2013; Chang et al., 2011; Johnston et al., 2012; Lyubomirsky, 2011; Purvis, Howell, & Iyer, 2011; Steger et al., 2006). It seems that exhaustively searching for optimal decision options may produce objectively better outcomes on a transient basis, but that such perfectionistic oriented individuals manifest increased post-decision regret and therefore lower levels of happiness than happy individuals, who tend to be more content with non-optimal solutions by ceasing to review the ramifications of already completed decisions. Happy individuals consistently manifest such a non-perfectionistic pattern of decision-making and post-decisional rationalization and contentment with a non-optimal decision through a protective pattern of decision response which tends to increase self-esteem by engaging in a dissonance-reducing post-decision reaction; this maintains positive emotions and self-worth (Lyubomirsky, 2011; Sharot, Fleming, Yu, Koster, & Dolan, 2012). Conversely, unhappy individuals’ post-decisional evaluations manifest dissatisfaction with a choice, irrespective of whether decisions were related to inconsequential or consequential matters.
Another factor influencing happiness levels relates to social comparisons with others. Happy individuals exhibit a relative insensitivity with regard to social comparisons, and thereby are able to maintain a positive emotional state in spite of the inevitable fluctuations of their own social conditions relative to others (Kassam, Morewedge, Gilbert, & Wilson, 2011; Lyubomirsky, Boehm, et al., 2011; Lyubomirsky & Tucker, 1998; Schwartz et al., 2002; Wood, 1989). It seems that the self-esteem and moods of happy individuals are somewhat impervious to peer comparisons. Also, happy individuals seem to be less involved with and concerned about micromanaging their emotional lives than unhappy individuals, who a tend to engage in lengthy ruminations reflecting on their own feelings, cognitions, and weaknesses (Bollich, Johannet, & Vazire, 2011; Koster, De Lissnyder, Derakshan, De Raedt, & Lissnyder, 2011; Lyubomirsky, Kasri, & Zehm, 2003; Michl, McLaughlin, Shepherd, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2013; Simsek, 2013; Verhaeghen, Joormann, & Aikman, 2014). This may appear to be counterintuitive, as many individuals believe that self-analysis and introspection are in fact beneficial keys to solving personal problems and even becoming enlightened. Therefore, although such self-reflective practices may increase self-knowledge and produce insight, they may do so at the cost of individual happiness. It may be that over-rumination involving prolonged self-reflection may involve a high cost and even be related to creating an intensifying depressive affect, and is therefore maladaptive from the viewpoint of personal happiness. Some evidence has shown that dispositionally happy individuals are less inclined to self-reflection (Lyubomirsky, Boehm, et al., 2011; Schwartz et al., 2002) and therefore may avoid some of these negative consequences, impairments, and cognitive pitfalls such as negative bias thinking, impaired concentration, dysphoria, decreased problem solving ability, and increased generalized stress levels (Brown & Brown, 2011). Happy individuals appear to be somewhat immune to self-reflection and introspection regarding the negative effects of failure by spending less time focusing on themselves (Kross et al., 2011; Stein & Grant, 2014; Tsai & Lau, 2013; Yoon & Joormann, 2012). Also, happy individuals use relatively fewer negatively biased words and experience relatively less negative thoughts in free association tasks (Hertel & Brozovich, 2010; Kashdan, Weeks, & Savostyanova, 2011; Kuo, Edge, & Ramel, 2011; Layous, Chancellor, Lyubomirsky, Wang, & Doraiswamy, 2011). Thus, it seems that unhappy individuals tend to spend more time dwelling on failure and associated memories from past failures, thereby impacting mood depression, self-confidence, and impairing performance and concentration. Happy individuals, though aware of failure, are able to compartmentalize negative experiences and minimize disappointment. As such, the cognitive processing strategies that characterize happy individuals avoid excessive self-reflection, which may usurp cognitive resources and precipitate self-perpetuating cycles of negative affect and diminished happiness.
One of the main alternative strategies utilized by happy individuals to avoid obsessive self-reflection is the constructive use of distraction and absorption in enjoyable activities and thoughts (Carpentier et al., 2011; Teismann, Michalak, Willutzki, & Schulte, 2012). Such constructive distractions may be particularly beneficial when the attention causes absorption and a sense of self transcendence (Csikszentmihalyi, 2014b). Attentional focus then seems to induce more positive moods than self-reflecting activities and is a highly effective strategy for improving mood and general happiness levels.
Obviously, individual responses to external events vary greatly, and response style is highly impactful on the level of happiness experienced (Lyubomirsky & Dickerhoof, 2011; Pe, Koval, & Kuppens, 2013). Happy individuals tend to interpret their life experiences more positively and their negative life experiences less negatively than unhappy individuals, and utilize different cognitive strategies such as humor or dwelling on avenues of improvement to respond to events. In other words, happy individuals manifest a proclivity toward positively biased cognition and a readiness to evaluate events, new situations, and people positively, thereby sustaining their individual happiness and increasing the probability of eliciting positive outcomes and responses from others.
Although happiness seems to be impacted by varying responses to life events, through hedonic adaptation happiness levels seem to reset to a particular point over time (Jacobs Bao, Lyubomirsky, & Bao, 2013; Layous & Lyubomirsky, 2012; Layous et al., 2011; Lyubomirsky, 2011; Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, 2006; Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, 2012; Uglanova & Staudinger, 2013). This certainly indicates that it may be at least difficult, if not impossible, for individuals to modify dispositional happiness, but fortunately, other research indicates that modification of levels of well-being and happiness is correlated with various motivational and cognitive factors yielding volitional strategies for self-transformation in therapeutic intervention (Caunt et al., 2013; Dijksterhuis & Aarts, 2010; Huang & Bargh, 2014; Kaczmarek et al., 2013; Lyubomirsky, Dickerhoof, et al., 2011; Morf & Horvath, 2010; Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, 2006; Tkach & Lyubomirsky, 2006; Vella-Brodrick, 2012). In fact numerous strategies are actually causal and amenable to direct manipulation, such as direct changes in commitment to activities, engaging in acts of kindness, and practicing gratitude (Vella-Brodrick, 2012).
A number of aspects Tibetan Buddhist thought are resonant with these aspects of positive psychology and CBT as numerous points of tangency exist between the explanation of the cause of afflictive emotions and the realization of enduring happiness (Ekman & Davidson, 2005; Kabat-Zinn, 2003; Rapgay & Bystrisky, 2009; Wallace, 2001). The aforementioned substantial body of studies on well-being set points indicates that even significant events only transiently alter individual states of happiness; individuals tend to return to their dispositional happiness set point. This coincides with the essence of the Tibetan Buddhist construct of sukha which essentially refers to a fundamental sense of well-being, compassion, reduced vulnerability to the vicissitudes of life, and an enhanced sense of interconnectedness with all living beings.
The Tibetan Buddhist and CBT systems both contain methods for modifying affective traits and cultivating long-term, enduring happiness through the cultivation of positive attributes of mind through continuing persistent effort. Both systems acutely focus on the interaction between emotion, thoughts, words, and actions. Both systems attempt to assist individuals in reducing suffering (duhkha) and achieving permanent happiness (sukha). Additionally, both CBT and Tibetan Buddhism posit that radical transformation of consciousness is necessary to realize these goals, and both systems strive to achieve emotional balance by learning to distinguish between subjective appearances and conceptual superimpositions as opposed to objective reality. Through proper training the goal for both is to perceive sense material and mental states in an undistorted manner free from subjective projections. Both systems attempt to realize mental states that are in actuality conducive to long-term well-being i.e. to employ skillful methods. Each system clearly distinguishes between affective states that are stimulated by transient physical and mental stimuli and a deeper awareness derived from mindful attention to emotion and cognition that ultimately creates an experience of happiness that is not contingent upon particular places, times, or circumstances. Both Tibetan Buddhism and CBT reject suppression as a tool to subdue destructive mental states and rather attempt to identify, through increased awareness, the manner in which they arise, are experienced, and their consequent emotional and mental impact. In other words, both require the cultivation and refinement of introspective abilities to monitor mental states and activities in order to enable the individual to become aware of disruptive and afflictive emotions and thoughts.
Individual Differences in Subjective Happiness
A purely objectivist view of happiness predicated on the idea that happy individuals are essentially those enjoying special advantages has been found to be lacking support (Diener & Scollon, 2014; Lyubomirsky, Dickerhoof, et al., 2011). In contrast, the theory that individuals create their own experiences via the manner in which they perceive, interpret, and remember actual experiences and events which then cause happiness has conversely found much support (Layous & Lyubomirsky, 2012; Lyubomirsky & Layous, 2013; Lyubomirsky, Dickerhoof, et al., 2011; Lyubomirsky, Sheldon, et al., 2005). Therefore, it seems that individual differences in happiness levels are directly linked, causally based on the manner in which individuals perceive, interpret, and view both major and minor life situations and events. Chronically happy and unhappy individuals ultimately experience life events in ways that support and bolster their affective dispositions. The cognitive mechanisms by which happiness is maintained have been identified, indicating that happy individuals experience less negative affect and more positive affect based situationally on judgments (Hellén & Sääksjärvi, 2011; Murakami, 2014). Additionally, (a) a sense of humor (Crawford & Caltabiano, 2011; Isen et al., 1987; Lyubomirsky & Tucker, 1998), (b) an ability to solve problems (Anderson, Goddard, & Powell, 2011), and (c) cognitive appraisal (Lee-Flynn, Pomaki, Delongis, Biesanz, & Puterman, 2011; Williams, Hundt, & Nelson-Gray, 2014; Zalewski, Lengua, Wilson, Trancik, & Bazinet, 2011) are effective strategies in moderating the effects of adverse and stressful life events and minimizing psychological distress. Happy individuals seem to be exceptionally skillful in relating to negative events and even turning those into positives. Obviously, it is almost certainly the case that causality is bidirectional, cyclical, and acting as a feedback loop with respect to happiness causing positive interpretations and cognitions about events as well as promoting a constructive construal of life events which then increases and maintains happiness. Ultimately, happy individuals seem to experience, interpret, and respond to external situations in ways that promote positive emotions and self-regard, and this manifests in decision-making and viewing the world and themselves positively in spite of contrary negative life events.
Summary
As a springboard for the research to be pursued in the next chapter, it may be of value to concisely summarize the material presented up to this point in the dissertation. First, the fundamental philosophic structures upon which the two key elements of the current study are based are noteworthy: REBT/CBT is deeply influenced by General Semantics and the MBTI is directly derived from Jungian psychological types. Additionally, both the MBTI and REBT/CBT have been assiduously analyzed and researched, clearly providing overwhelming evidence of substantial philosophical and psychological value. In particular, REBT/CBT has been found to be impressively useful in treating multitudinous clinical problems. However, as reflected in the previous discussion, it is not a panacea. Therefore, the study to be pursued here examines the question of whether there exists a simple and direct method for utilizing the MBTI predicatively to ascertain proactively the utility of REBT/CBT for an arbitrary individual. Additionally analyzed, and possibly more importantly, is the possibility of utilizing the MBTI to determine if in fact REBT/CBT may even produce deleterious effects for a given individual. These issues represent the focus of the study to be pursued in Chapter 3.
Chapter 3 – Method
Purpose of the Study
This study was designed to test the hypothesis that a relationship exists between Jungian psychological types, as measured by the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator of personality variables, and the utility of Cognitive Behavioral therapy restructuring methods (See Appendix D) to produce psychological growth. This was measured by the following assessments: Subjective Happiness Scale, Personal Growth Initiative Scale, Meaning in Life Questionnaire, Satisfaction with Life Scale, and Gratitude Questionnaire (See Appendix A).
Design
All students from my introductory computer science classes at Santa Monica College were included in the study. Hence, no exclusion criteria were utilized with respect to ethnicity, gender, race, or age. The reason for such broad inclusion is that such classes represent a wide-ranging spectrum of diversity within these categories. The sample consisted of 281 students (149 females and 132 males). Subjects were told that they were taking part in a learning experiment by way of a consent form (See Appendix B).
A MBTI test was administered at the beginning of the study by means of a spreadsheet (See Appendix C). As a result of the MBTI assessment, all participants were classified on a percentage basis from 0 to 100 according to four dichotomous dimensions: 1. Extraversion-Introversion, 2. Sensing-Intuition, 3. Thinking-Feeling, and 4.Judging-Perceiving. Hence, sixteen weighted type classifications were possible (ISTJ, ISFJ, INFJ, INTJ, ISTP, ISFP, INFP, INTP, ESTP, ESFP, ENFP, ENTP, ESTJ, ESFJ, ENFJ, ENTJ).
After ascertaining individual psychological type, questionnaires were administered via Surveymonkey for each of the six positive psychology questionnaires used in the study (See Appendix A). Subjects were then exposed by means of readings and written summaries to CBT/REBT concepts throughout a 5 week period (See Appendix D). Finally, the six positive psychology questionnaires were administered for a second time to determine the effect of exposure to CBT concepts.
Confidentiality
Since confidentiality and the protection of privacy of research subjects is of utmost importance, all data submitted have been stored for the duration of the study on my personal computer in an encrypted state in password protected files. Physical access to this computer is completely restricted. Additionally, the minimum necessary participant identification was collected. Although the names of individuals taking the Myers-Briggs spreadsheet assessment were collected, all data collected for this project has been reported without any identifying information. The questionnaires submitted to Surveymonkey do not have any identifying information. Additionally, after data collection, deidentification and confidentiality were ensured through deletion of all materials submitted after data consolidation.
Statistical Analysis
The data collected was subjected to numerous and varied statistical tests. The spreadsheet used to categorize subjects into the 16 Meyers Briggs typologies was subjected to a two sample F-Test for variances followed by a mean-matched pairs test to compare 3 groups from the study (all, females, males) to standard population percentages derived from the full MBTI test. Additionally, a correlation and regression test was implemented to further validate the use of the abbreviated version of the personality test.
Next, after confirming approximately equal variances (homoscedasticity) via Bartlett’s test, an ANOVA test was applied to the before and after difference of the normalized results for the all three categories in the study (‘all subjects’, ‘females’, and ‘males’) to determine whether differences existed in the 16 MBTI types in response to the CBT concepts exposure. After ascertaining whether outcomes were asymmetric by way of an ANOVA test, a two sample F-Test for equality of variances and a Ryan-Joiner test for data normality was applied to each of the 16 types in each of the ‘all’, females’, and ‘males’ categories to justify the use of a dependent mean-matched pairs T-test on the same 48 sets of data. Again, tests were applied to normalized data.
Finally, each of the 8 dichotomous MBTI categories (extroversion, introversion, sensing, intuition, thinking, feeling, judging, and perceiving) was individually analyzed with respect to strength relative to the continuum of opposite poles (e.g. 100 extroversion to 100 introversion) for subjects in the first and fourth quartiles (I>75%, I<25% etc.). After confirming homoscedasticity via Bartlett’s test, an ANOVA test was applied to the before and after difference of the normalized results for all 16 categories to investigate differential effects. This was followed by statistical scrutiny by way of a two sample F-Test for equality of variances and a Ryan-Joiner test for data normality which was followed by a mean-matched test applied to the before and after normalized data difference.
The results of these statistical investigations are given in detail in Appendix F and are summarized in the next chapter.
Chapter 4 – Summary of Data Analysis
Although this summary of the data analysis from Appendix F may seem unwieldy, the final results and conclusions are in fact straightforward and simple. First, an analysis was given of the validity of the abridged personality test given to the subjects (all subjects, females, and males) by way of a spreadsheet (See Appendix C) relative to the full version of the MBTI. After the applicability of a T-test applied to before and after data was indicated by a two sample F-Test for variances and a Ryan-Joiner test for normality (both significance levels < .01), a T-test was performed at a 99% confidence level for the data for all three groups indicating a statistically significant correlation (p < .01) with the full version of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. In addition, a Correlation and Regression test was applied to all three categories and yielded a coefficient of determination > .99 in all cases.
Next, after confirming homoscedasticity via Bartlett’s test (df=15, sig level =.05) an ANOVA test on the normalized data differences to ascertain differential effects from CBT exposure for all three categories (‘All Subjects’, ‘Females’, and ‘Males’) indicated that there were statistically significant differences (p <.01). Following these results, each of the 16 MBTI combinations for the ‘all subjects’, ‘females’, and ‘males’ categories were analyzed to determine which ones experienced changes from the CBT exposure. For each of these 48 categories a two sample F-Test for variances and a Ryan-Joiner test for normality were utilized to justify the application of a T-test. All 48 categories were tested at a .01 significance level and were found to be approximately normal and exhibiting homoscedasticity. Each of the 48 categories were then subjected to a before/after mean-matched pairs test which yielded the following results: two categories (ISTJ and INTJ) showed statistically significant positive transformation (p < .01) from the CBT exposure in each of the subject categories (all, female, and male), and all other cases indicated that there was no significant change in any of the remaining 42 categories i.e. all other groups showed no improvement and no adverse effects from the CBT concept exposure.
The fact that only ISTJ and INTJ yielded any change, led to the hypothesis that I, T, and J may be individually responsible for positive transformation. An investigation into all 8 dichotomous dimensions based on strength of personality type (i.e. >75% or <25%) was therefore pursued. First, a Bartlett’s Test was done to confirm homoscedasticity (significance level = 5%) followed by an ANOVA applied to the ‘all subjects’ category for each of the 16 strength categories (I>75%, I<25%, etc.), which resulted in differences being indicated (p < .01). Each of these 16 categories was then tested at a 5% significance level via a two sample F-test for variances and a Ryan-Joiner test for normality to justify the application of a T test. As all 16 categories were tested at a .01 significance level and were found to be approximately normal and homoscedastic, a T-test was applied to the before and after normalized data which indicated that I > 75%, T > 75%, and J > 75% demonstrated statistically significant improvement (p < .02) after CBT concept exposure, and that all other categories demonstrated no change at a 5% significance level. 1
Summarizing, consistent with expectations these results indicate that with respect to change from CBT exposure, clear differences exist among the 16 Myers-Briggs types. In particular, two typologies (ISTJ and INTJ) are strongly correlated with successful outcomes as measured by the happiness and goal assessment questionnaires. Additionally, the results indicate that no typologies experienced negative impact from exposure to CBT/REBT concepts. In other words, at worst, 14 typologies demonstrated no progress from concept exposure. Finally, 3 individual personality dimensions (I > 75%, T > 75%, and J > 75%) manifested clear positive change from the CBT concept exposure. The implications of these results are next discussed in chapter 5.
Chapter 5 – Conclusion and Recommendations
This section gives a summary of the conclusions derived from the statistical findings given in Appendix F and Chapter 4, considers limitations of the study, and makes recommendations for further research. The underlying theoretical formulations of cognitive behavioral therapy and the Myers-Briggs type indicator assessment, specifically Korzybski’s General Semantics (Ellis, 2007; Korzybski, 1994; Levinson, 2010) and Jungian personality types (Capraro & Capraro, 2002; Carlyn, 1977; Corr, 1996; Karn, Syed-Abdullah, Cowling, & Holcombe, 2007; Myers, & Myers, 2010; Saggino & Kline, 1996) respectively, provide a firm philosophic, theoretical substructure that support their utilitarian value in research and psychotherapeutic application. Nevertheless, clear, simple, and practical clinical results have also been implicated in this pilot study. In fact, the research presented in this dissertation was conducted to investigate the utility of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator as a predictor of success or failure in the use of Cognitive Behavioral Therapy and Rational Emotive Behavioral Therapy in clinical psychology by quantitatively testing the hypothesis that there exists a relationship between Jungian psychological types, as measured by the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator of personality variables and, after exposure to Cognitive Behavioral Therapy concepts, psychological growth measured by the following: Subjective Happiness Scale, Personal Growth Initiative Scale, Meaning in Life Questionnaire, Satisfaction with Life Scale , and Gratitude Questionnaire. However, the current study does not represent actual hypothesis testing and therefore the efficacy of the correlations which have emerged are preliminary and in no way generalize beyond the present study. As this pilot study represents an initial stage in the design and implementation of the overall research process, larger scale studies could prove fruitful.
The most salient results of the current study are that ISTJ, INTJ, I > 75%, T > 75%, and J > 75% demonstrate constructive change. This was derived from the fact that both dependent matched-means tests for the 16 MBTI types before and after subject groups and independent matched-means tests for the independent categories E-I, S-N, T-F, and P-J indicated an overwhelmingly positive relationship between these particular personality variables and development, as measured by the various questionnaires mentioned above and administered to subjects after exposure to basic cognitive restructuring concepts. The use of these methodologies permitted a clear analysis of the patterns of Myers-Briggs personality and improved outlook. The general pattern of findings provides evidence that personality type may be highly influential with respect to improvement of outlook derived from cognitive behavioral therapy. Hence, not only was the initial hypothesis, simply relating to the fact that differences in the 16 personality types would exist validated, but potentially a very simple method for predicting the utility of cognitive behavioral restructuring methods through solely finding the strengths of the I, T, and J types unexpectedly emerged. These results suggest that a simple clinical test of personality type may be utilized to decide therapeutic direction for this modality. Clearly this suggests a method for maximizing the effectiveness of the utilization of this method in psychotherapeutic clinical settings. The present study indicates that it may be quite advantageous to consider the various Jungian type manifestations since they are most likely so impactful on the outcomes of rational emotive behavior therapy and cognitive behavioral therapy. When personality typology indicates that a particular individual would most likely benefit by these modalities, it is highly desirable to utilize such methods; otherwise the results indicate that these approaches may be effectively a waste of time.
In one sense the results of the current study should not be surprising. Even casual consideration of the three types that manifested strongly constructive transformation as measured by the psychological growth surveys lends support to this view. Prior research (Campbell & Heller, 1987; Edwards, 2003; Edwards, Lanning, & Hooker, 2002; Lorr, 1991) on these personality dimensions indicates that ‘introverted-thinking’ typologies are inclined to use exact wording to express ideas, notice minute differentiations in the underlying definition of things, and are prone to analysis and classification. Also, they tend to examine every facet of an issue and attempt to construct systems and models which are free from logical inconsistency to solve generalized problems while at the same time minimizing risk and effort. Introverted thinking types zealously endeavor to find a precise wording to unambiguously express ideas in as concise a manner as possible (Beebe, 2012; Cahill & Polich, 1992). Internally they make every effort to engage the fundamental qualities and fine distinctions that lie at the essence of an object or subject before categorizing or naming it. Hence, they are profoundly inclined to internal reasoning in the derivation of classes and subcategories, general principles and subsidiary principles. Their systems are used in analysis, problem-solving, and the evolution of ideas by considering multifaceted issues and flushing out inconsistencies. This requires constant vigilance with respect to logical inconsistencies regarding fundamental frameworks and systemic axiomatic statements in the pursuit of models used to evaluate the universe at large. Judging types usually employ planned structures and organization in daily tasks utilizing guidelines and fixed methods to complete goal directed tasks in a step-by-step manner, and thinking types are inclined to utilize logic and principles to arrive at decisions dispassionately and rationally (Campbell & Heller, 1987; Edwards et al., 2002; Lorr, 1991). REBT and CBT are obviously attractive to and would be ideal candidate systems for such types.
The present results also suggest that correlations may be found not only between the Myers-Briggs type indicator and specific psychotherapeutic modalities, but also between other personality assessments such as self-report inventories (MMPI-2, Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire, California Personality Inventory, NEO Personality Inventory), projective personality tests (Rorschach test, Thematic Apperception Test), the five-factor model, Cattell’s 16PF Trait Theory, Allport’s Trait Theory, (Bouchard, McGue, Hur, & Horn, 1998; Kasser et al., 2014; King et al., 1999; McCrae & Costa, 1997; McCrae et al., 1999; Sherman, Nave, & Funder, 2010; Steger & Kashdan, 2008) and the numerous varieties of psychotherapeutic theories of personality and concomitant interventions.2 In other words, the results presented here suggest that practical targeted assessment screening tests may have remarkable utility in ascertaining the viability of a particular modality to be utilized in a clinical setting. It may be feasible, appropriate, and even essential to select intervention strategies based on simple personality assessments rather than leaving it to psychotherapists’ intuitive heuristics. Instead, utilizing the actuality of strong correlations between assessments and treatment modalities, clinicians may more accurately guide clients to the proper type of treatment rather than relying on their ‘default mode’ or favorite system by simply administering a personality assessment and guiding the client to the indicated associated treatment modality. Any systematic method of choosing a client appropriate type of therapy would necessarily facilitate consciousness growth. Of course, before implementing a program based on the results of this pilot study, obviously further research is in order to probe and validate the results of this dissertation in particular and more generally to investigate other psychological assessments and relationships to varieties of psychotherapeutic modalities.
Criticisms of the Study
Of course, as with nearly all research this study has limitations. Issues concerning the generalizability of the findings are raised by the use of the community college students who served as subjects for the study. In fact, this may be the primary weakness given that subjects were chosen from a highly restricted group of community college students with an interest in technology, mostly under the age of 25 years old. Additionally, the pool of subjects was not seeking ‘treatment’. This obviously places limits on the generalizability of the findings. Future research may possibly attempt to rectify this through the utilization of a more diverse pool of subjects in terms of age, educational interest, gender, socioeconomic status, race, ethnicity, income, occupation, and religious affiliation, and spirituality. Research of this nature might allow further elucidation of the processes by which the expression of happiness behaviors may be measured and realized.
Another limitation of the current study is the fact that the distribution of individuals in both the study and in the general population in each of the 16 MBTI groups is not uniform and therefore, in spite of the total of 281 subjects altogether, some groups consisted of only a few individuals and therefore conclusions may be subject to statistical error. One solution to this may be to do a study on a much broader scale to wash this effect out. Additionally, methodological issues must also be noted with regards to the self-selection to opt into the study, and also by the fact that subjects were given extra credit for participating.
Another fundamental criticism of the study is related to the fact that as an assessment device the MBTI is not without criticism (Boyle, 1995; Capraro & Capraro, 2002; Lorr, 1991). The rigid classifications of such tests as the Myers-Briggs type indicator has been criticized in the light of Jung’s views on psychological types by warnings that such objective scoring necessarily leads to fallacious assumptions and conclusions (Boyle, 1995; Bradway & Detloff, 1976; Carlyn, 1977; Corr, 1996). Also, the utility of measuring Jungian psychological types through the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator has been criticized due to its dichotomous scoring and forced-choice response formats (Boyle, 1995; Capik & Gozum, 2014; Harvey & Murry, 2010; Ried, 2014; Vispoel & Kim, 2014; Webb, 1964). Differential weighting of item responses and alternative measurements have been suggested such as the Personal Preferences Self-Description Questionnaire (Ghaderi, 2005; Kier, Melancon, & Thompson, 1998; Leach, 2006; Light, Joyce, & Frampton, 2009; Marsh, Ellis, Parada, Richards, & Heubeck, 2005; Sarafino & Graham, 2006; Vacha-Haase, Thompson, & Vacha-Haase, 2002). The Singer-Loomis Type Deployment Inventory, Personal Preferences Self-Description Questionnaire and the five-factor model have also been proposed as alternatives to Jungian personality measurement of psychometric properties (Andreassen et al., 2013; Arnau, Rosen, & Thompson, 2000; Bjørnebekk et al., 2013; Furnham, 1996; Gore & Widiger, 2013; Kristjansson, 2012; Lorr, 1991; MacDonald, 1995; McAdams, 1992; Murray & Russ, 1981; Samuel, South, & Griffin, 2015; Saucier, 1994; Trull, 2012; Walker & Jackson, 2014; Widiger & Presnall, 2013; Wiggins & Schwartz, 1991).
In addition to these findings, even if the MBTI assessment is hypothesized as a valid predictor of psychological types, some studies indicate little or no utility in some domains. For example, one study showed that the selection of psychotherapeutic orientation and personality variables related to student enrollment in theoretical counseling courses showed no statistically significant relationship for preference of theory as measured by the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (Freeman, Hayes, Kuch, & Taub, 2007) and the relationship between environmental circumstances and internal personal attributes as they relate to job satisfaction indicates that MBTI psychological types may be unrelated to job satisfaction (Thomas, Buboltz, & Winkelspecht, 2004). Another study generally investigated the relationship between self-knowledge, wisdom and maturity, and balancing polarities of the MBTI psychological types. A subtle analysis of the strengths and weaknesses of preferences and concomitant limitations was delineated concluding that no one-dimensional behavioral guide may be derived from a simplistic analysis of type preferences (Boyle, 1995; Capraro & Capraro, 2002; Edwards, 2003; Lloyd, 2012(a)). However, the remarkable results of the current pilot study demonstrating strong correlations between two MBTI types and improved outlook from CBT concept exposure diverge from these opposing studies and lend support to the superabundance of previously mentioned research supporting the predictive value of the MBTI.
A final criticism is that the application of a horizontal treatment modality such as CBT may not be useful when addressing issues in the vertical dimension. In fact, this may ironically be the case in regard to Analytical Psychology. As positive and negative archetypal opposites necessarily coexist, manifesting in both light and dark aspects, Jung believed that a union of opposites was necessary to avoid enantiodromia (psychic imbalance due to the unconscious development of a polar opposite). In fact, he conceived that enlightenment and individuation are possible only by harmonizing and ultimately transcending dualities. The balancing of dualities does not come about through repression of a polar opposite, but rather by experiencing, through awareness and acceptance, both the light and dark aspects of each archetype. From this perspective, within Analytical Psychology or any modality focused on the vertical dimension of psychological growth and development, it may be necessary to utilize psychological type and the innate drive towards wholeness and well-being to facilitate deeper psychic structural change and spiritual transformation within a multidimensional context.
Future Research
Despite the aforementioned limitations, the preponderance of studies previously mentioned in this dissertation indicates that the MBTI is a research assessment tool of longstanding value. Hence, the conclusions of the present research validly suggest numerous courses for future research and practice. In accord with much research in positive psychology (Frisch, 2013; Green, Oades, & Grant, 2006; Henderson, Knight, & Richardson, 2013; Landor, 2013; Mak, Ng, & Wong, 2011; Niven, Totterdell, Holman, & Headley, 2012; O’Brien, 2012; Rusk & Waters, 2014; Schueller, 2011; Steger & Kashdan, 2013), the present study indicates that certain types of individuals can undoubtedly influence their own well-being via cognitive restructuring methods. Hence, additionally, the present results shed light on the theoretical philosophic positions advocated by those who hold the viewpoint of automatic processing (Baumeister, Crescioni, & Alquist, 2011; Ebert & Wegner, 2011; Ent & Baumeister, 2014; McKenna, 2012; Pronin & Kugler, 2010; Simonton, 2013), as well as the practical application by therapists attempting to not only alleviate suffering but increase happiness in clients. As alluded to in the literature review, even though individuals seem to carry automatic default response mechanisms (Bargh, Chen, & Burrows, 1996; Bargh, Lombardi, & Higgins, 1988; Bargh, Schwader, Hailey, Dyer, & Boothby, 2012; Bargh & Ferguson, 2000; Bargh, 2011; Huang & Bargh, 2014; Macrae & Johnston, 1998; McConnell, Shoda, & Skulborstad, 2012; Orbell & Verplanken, 2010; Ockenfels, & Mussweiler, 2014; Teachman, Joormann, Steinman, & Gotlib, 2012), it at least appears that a few targeted types are able to realize transformation through cognitive restructuring (Ehlers et al., 2010; Fisk, 2010; Lyubomirsky, Boehm, Kasri, & Zehm, 2011). Additionally, even though the literature review gave ample consideration to the possibility of automaticity of cognitive processes, the fact that some categories realized constructive change, through concept exposure alone, diminishes the likelihood of the automatic processing perspective being ultimately true. The fact that constructive change was produced in the current research lends support to the fundamental claims of positive psychology relative to the possibility of self-transformation which advocate the viability of utilizing specific transformational strategies to effect psychological well-being (Crawford & Caltabiano, 2011; Fredrickson, 2001; Layous, Chancellor, Lyubomirsky, Wang, & Doraiswamy, 2011; Lyubomirsky, King, & Diener, 2005; Rashid, 2014; Schueller & Seligman, 2010; Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, 2012; Tay & Diener, 2011; Thoen & Robitschek, 2013; Tugade & Fredrickson, 2004). Follow up studies to this dissertation may hopefully attempt to further validate the findings presented here and possibly establish a broader based link between personality assessments as a predictor and improvement based, not only on psychological growth measured by the scales such as those utilized in this study, but other metrics such as IQ tests, achievement tests, employment tests, attitude tests, objective personality tests (Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory, Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-III, and the Beck Depression Inventory), projective tests, interest tests, and aptitude tests (Capraro & Capraro, 2002; Gámez, Chmielewski, Kotov, Ruggero, & Watson, 2011; Huang & Bargh, 2014; Kahneman & Krueger, 2006; Kashdan et al., 2013; Lyubomirsky et al., 2011; McCrae et al., 1997; Pavot, Diener, Colvin, & Sandvik, 1991; Staw, Sutton, & Pelled, 1994). Additionally, these research extensions would shed light on other treatment approachesincluding clinical social work, marriage and family therapy, psychiatric nursing, sex therapy, religious or spiritual counseling, pastoral counseling, and life coaching.
In conclusion, my hope is that findings from the present thesis will pave the way for future research investigating the various dimensions of happiness and its relation to personality assessment variables. Advances in these domains may inform conceptual models of happiness and also lead to improved, focused therapeutic treatment in clinical psychology. As with the analysis of any constructs related to human beings, there may be no simplistic formula for achieving happiness. It may be that the complex amalgamation of goals, self-evaluations, values, feelings, and behaviors is beyond the reach of any formulaic, algorithmic, scientific analysis and research. And yet, the search seems to be appropriate even if it is arduous or endless.
References
Abbey, A. (1987). Perceptions of Personal Avoidability Versus Responsibility: How Do They Differ? Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 8(1), 3–19. doi:10.1207/s15324834basp0801&2_2
Adams, E. M. (1974). Linguistic Analysis and Epistemic Encounters. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 34(3), 404. doi:10.2307/2107086
Aghajani, M., Veer, I. I. M., van Tol, M.-J., Aleman, A., van Buchem, M. A., Veltman, D. J., … Tol, M. van. (2013). Neuroticism and extraversion are associated with amygdala resting-state functional connectivity. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14(2), 836–48. doi:10.3758/s13415-013-0224-0
Allen, A. A. K., Wilkins, K., Gazzaley, A., & Morsella, E. (2013). Conscious thoughts from reflex-like processes: a new experimental paradigm for consciousness research. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(4), 1318–31. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2013.08.007
Allison, H. E. (2011). Transcendental Realism, Empirical Realism and Transcendental Idealism. Kantian Review, 11, 1–28. doi:10.1017/S1369415400002223
Amin, N., Foa, E. E. B., & Coles, M. E. (1998). Negative interpretation bias in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(10), 945–957. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00060-6
Amir, N., Taylor, C. T. C., & Donohue, M. C. M. (2011). Predictors of response to an attention modification program in generalized social phobia. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79(4), 533–41. doi:10.1037/a0023808
Anderson, R. J., Goddard, L., & Powell, J. H. (2011). Social problem-solving and depressive symptom vulnerability: The importance of real-life problem-solving performance. Cognitive therapy and research, 35(1), 48-56.
Andreassen, C. S., Griffiths, M. D., Gjertsen, S. R., Krossbakken, E., Kvam, S., & Pallesen, S. (2013). The relationships between behavioral addictions and the five-factor model of personality. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 2(2), 90–99. doi:10.1556/JBA.2.2013.003
Angeles, L. (2011). A closer look at the Easterlin paradox. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 40(1), 67–73. doi:10.1016/j.socec.2010.06.017
Angner, E. (2011). The evolution of eupathics: The historical roots of subjective measures of well-being. International Journal of Wellbeing, 1(1). doi:10.5502/ijw.v1i1.14
Archontaki, D., Lewis, G. J., & Bates, T. C. (2013). Genetic Influences on Psychological Well-Being: A Nationally Representative Twin Study. Journal of Personality, 81(2), 221–230. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2012.00787.x
Arnau, R., Green, B., & Rosen, D. (2003). Are Jungian preferences really categorical?: An empirical investigation using taxometric analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 34(2), 233–251. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00040-5
Arnau, R. C., Rosen, D. H., & Thompson, B. (2000). Reliability and validity of scores from the Singer-Loomis Type Deployment Inventory. Journal of Analytical Psychology, 45(3), 409–426. doi:10.1111/1465-5922.00173
Ashbaugh, A. R., & Radomsky, A. S. (2009). Memory for Physiological Feedback in Social Anxiety Disorder: The Role of Fear of Bodily Sensations. Cogn Ther Res, 35(4), 304–316. doi:10.1007/s10608-009-9291-5
Ashkanasy, N. M., & Humphrey, R. H. R. (2011). Current Emotion Research in Organizational Behavior. Emotion Review, 3(2), 214–224. doi:10.1177/1754073910391684
Bachmann, T. (2012). How to Begin to Overcome the Ambiguity Present in Differentiation between Contents and Levels of Consciousness? Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 82. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00082
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13(1), 27-45.
Bailey, R., & Wells, A. (2014). Metacognitive Therapy in the Treatment of Hypochondriasis: A Systematic Case Series. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 38(5), 541–550. doi:10.1007/s10608-014-9615-y
Bailey, T. C., & Snyder, C. R. (2010). Satisfaction with life and hope: A look at age and marital status. The Psychological Record, 57(2), 5.
Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Fishman, D. D. J. F., Mrazek, M. D., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Unnoticed intrusions: Dissociations of meta-consciousness in thought suppression. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(3), 1003–12. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2013.06.009
Baldwin, D. D. S., Waldman, S., & Allgulander, C. (2011). Evidence-based pharmacological treatment of generalized anxiety disorder. The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology / Official Scientific Journal of the Collegium Internationale Neuropsychopharmacologicum (CINP), 14(5), 697–710. doi:10.1017/S1461145710001434
Bao, K. J. K. (2012). The Course of Well-Being in Romantic Relationships: Predicting Positive Affect in Dating Participants. Psychology, 03(12), 1091–1099. doi:10.4236/psych.2012.312A161
Bargh, J. A. (1988). Automatic information processing: Implications for communication and affect. Communication, social cognition, and affect, 9, 37.
Bargh, J. J. A. (2011). Unconscious Thought Theory and Its Discontents: A Critique of the Critiques. Social Cognition, 29(6), 629–647. doi:10.1521/soco.2011.29.6.629
Bargh, J. J. A. (2013). Social psychology cares about causal conscious thought, not free will per se. The British Journal of Social Psychology / the British Psychological Society, 52(2), 228–30. doi:10.1111/bjso.12011
Bargh, J. J. A., Bond, R. R. N., Lombardi, W. J., & Tota, M. E. (1986). The additive nature of chronic and temporary sources of construct accessibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(5), 869–878. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.50.5.869
Bargh, J. J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 230–244. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.230
Bargh, J. J. A., & Ferguson, M. J. M. (2000). Beyond behaviorism: On the automaticity of higher mental processes. Psychological Bulletin, 126(6), 925–945. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.6.925
Bargh, J. J. A., Lombardi, W. J. W., & Higgins, E. T. E. (1988). Automaticity of chronically accessible constructs in person × situation effects on person perception: It’s just a matter of time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(4), 599–605. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.55.4.599
Bargh, J. J. A., Schwader, K. L. K., Hailey, S. S. E., Dyer, R. L., & Boothby, E. J. (2012). Automaticity in social-cognitive processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(12), 593–605. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.002
Batink, T., Peeters, F., Geschwind, N., van Os, J., & Wichers, M. (2013). How does MBCT for depression work? studying cognitive and affective mediation pathways. PloS One, 8(8), e72778. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0072778
Batmaz, S., Kaymak, S. U., Soygur, A. H., Ozalp, E., & Turkcapar, M. H. (2013). The distinction between unipolar and bipolar depression: A cognitive theory perspective. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 54(7), 740–749. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2013.02.004
Baumeister, R. F., Crescioni, A. W., & Alquist, J. L. (2010). Free Will as Advanced Action Control for Human Social Life and Culture. Neuroethics, 4(1), 1–11. doi:10.1007/s12152-010-9058-4
Bayne, R. (1995). The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator: A critical review and practical guide. Nelson Thornes.
Beard, C. (2011). Cognitive bias modification for anxiety: current evidence and future directions. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics, 11(2), 299–311. doi:10.1586/ern.10.194
Beard, C., & Amir, N. (2010). Negative Interpretation Bias Mediates the Effect of Social Anxiety on State Anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 34(3), 292–296. doi:10.1007/s10608-009-9258-6
Beard, C., Moitra, E., Weisberg, R. B., & Keller, M. B. (2010). Characteristics and predictors of social phobia course in a longitudinal study of primary‐care patients. Depression and Anxiety, 27(9), 839–45. doi:10.1002/da.20676
Beatty, M. J. (1988). Impact of ambiguity reduction about performance expectations on audience anxiety.Communication Education, 37(3), 208–213. doi:10.1080/03634528809378720
Beck, A. A. T., & Dozois, D. D. J. A. (2011). Cognitive therapy: current status and future directions. Annual Review of Medicine, 62(1), 397–409. doi:10.1146/annurev-med-052209-100032
Beck, A. A. T., & Haigh, E. A. P. E. (2014). Advances in cognitive theory and therapy: the generic cognitive model. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 10(1), 1–24. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153734
Beebe, J. (2012). Psychological Types in Freud and Jung. Jung Journal, 6(3), 58–71. doi:10.1525/jung.2012.6.3.58
Beevers, C. G., Wenzlaff, R. M., Hayes, A. M., & Scott, W. D. (2006). Depression and the Ironic Effects of Thought Suppression: Therapeutic Strategies for Improving Mental Control. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 6(2), 133–148. doi:10.1093/clipsy.6.2.133
Beltman, M. W. M., Voshaar, R. R. C. O., & Speckens, A. A. E. (2010). Cognitive-behavioural therapy for depression in people with a somatic disease: meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. The British Journal of Psychiatry : The Journal of Mental Science, 197(1), 11–9. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.109.064675
Berking, M., Ebert, D., Cuijpers, P., & Hofmann, S. G. (2013). Emotion regulation skills training enhances the efficacy of inpatient cognitive behavioral therapy for major depressive disorder: a randomized controlled trial. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 82(4), 234–45. doi:10.1159/000348448
Beyler, J., & Schmeck, R. R. (1992). Assessment of Individual Differences in Preferences for Holistic-Analytic Strategies: Evaluation of Some Commonly Available Instruments. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 52(3), 709–719. doi:10.1177/0013164492052003021
Bhaskar, R. (2000). Introducing Transcendental Dialectical Critical Realism. Alethia, 3(1), 15–21. doi:10.1558/aleth.v3i1.15
Birditt, K. K. S., Hope, S., Brown, E., & Orbuch, T. (2012). Developmental Trajectories of Marital Happiness Over 16 Years. Research in Human Development, 9(2), 126–144. doi:10.1080/15427609.2012.680844
Biswas-Diener, R., & Patterson, L. (2010). Positive Psychology and Poverty. Positive Psychology as Social Change, 125–140. doi:10.1007/978-90-481-9938-9_8
Bittencourt, G. (2007). Combining Syntax and Semantics through Prime Form Representation. Journal of Logic and Computation, 18(1), 13–33. doi:10.1093/logcom/exm051
Bjørnebekk, A., Fjell, A. M., Walhovd, K. B., Grydeland, H., Torgersen, S., & Westlye, L. T. (2013). Neuronal correlates of the five factor model (FFM) of human personality: Multimodal imaging in a large healthy sample. NeuroImage, 65, 194–208. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.009
Blackwell, S. E. S., & Holmes, E. E. A. (2010). Modifying interpretation and imagination in clinical depression: A single case series using cognitive bias modification. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(3), 338–350. doi:10.1002/acp.1680
Blanchflower, D. G. D. D. G., & Oswald, A. A. J. (2011). International Happiness: A New View on the Measure of Performance. Academy of Management Perspectives, 25(1), 6–22. doi:10.5465/AMP.2011.59198445
Bogaert, S., Boone, C., & Declerck, C. (2008). Social value orientation and cooperation in social dilemmas: a review and conceptual model. The British Journal of Social Psychology / the British Psychological Society, 47(Pt 3), 453–80. doi:10.1348/014466607X244970
Bollich, K. L. K., Johannet, P. M. P., & Vazire, S. (2011). In search of our true selves: feedback as a path to self-knowledge. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 312. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00312
Bond, F. W., & Dryden, W. (2000). How rational beliefs and irrational beliefs affect people’s inferences: An experimental investigation. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 28(01), 33-43.
Bond, F. W., Dryden, W., & Briscoe, R. (1999). Testing two mechanisms by which rational and irrational beliefs may affect the functionality of inferences. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 72(4), 557–566. doi:10.1348/000711299160121
Boterhoven De Haan, K. L., & Lee, C. W. (2014). Therapists’ thoughts on therapy: clinicians’ perceptions of the therapy processes that distinguish schema, cognitive behavioural and psychodynamic approaches. Psychotherapy Research : Journal of the Society for Psychotherapy Research, 24(5), 538–49. doi:10.1080/10503307.2013.861092
Bouchard, T. J., McGue, M., Hur, Y.-M., & Horn, J. M. (1998). A genetic and environmental analysis of the California Psychological Inventory using adult twins reared apart and together. European Journal of Personality, 12(5), 307–320. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0984(1998090)12:5<307::AID-PER336>3.0.CO;2-K
Boyle, G. J. (1995). Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI): Some Psychometric Limitations. Australian Psychologist, 30(1), 71–74. doi:10.1111/j.1742-9544.1995.tb01750.x
Bradway, K., & Detloff, W. (1976). Incidence of Psychological Types. Journal of Analytical Psychology, 21(2), 134–146. doi:10.1111/j.1465-5922.1976.00134.x
Branch, R., & Willson, R. (2010). Cognitive behavioural therapy. John Wiley & Sons.
Brannan, D., Biswas-Diener, R., Mohr, C. D., Mortazavi, S., & Stein, N. (2013). Friends and family: A cross-cultural investigation of social support and subjective well-being among college students. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 8(1), 65–75. doi:10.1080/17439760.2012.743573
Brauhardt, A., de Zwaan, M., Herpertz, S., Zipfel, S., Svaldi, J., Friederich, H. C., & Hilbert, A. (2014). Therapist adherence in individual cognitive-behavioral therapy for binge-eating disorder: Assessment, course, and predictors. Behaviour research and therapy, 61, 55-60.
Brett, C. C. E., Gow, A. A. J., Corley, J., Pattie, A., Starr, J. M., & Deary, I. J. (2012). Psychosocial factors and health as determinants of quality of life in community-dwelling older adults. Quality of Life Research : An International Journal of Quality of Life Aspects of Treatment, Care and Rehabilitation, 21(3), 505–16. doi:10.1007/s11136-011-9951-2
Bridges, K. R. K., & Harnish, R. (2010). Role of irrational beliefs in depression and anxiety: a review. Health, 02(08), 862–877. doi:10.4236/health.2010.28130
Briggs-Myers, I., McCaulley, M. H., Quenk, N. L., & Hammer, A. L. (2003). MBTI manual. A guide to the development and use of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. 3rd Edition. Menlo Park, CA: CPP Inc.
Brockmann, H., & Yan, S. (2013). My Car is Bigger than Yours: Consumption, Status Competition, and Happiness in Times of Affluence. Happiness Studies Book Series, 131–147. doi:10.1007/978-94-007-6609-9_10
Brosan, L., Hoppitt, L., Shelfer, L., Sillence, A., & Mackintosh, B. (2011). Cognitive bias modification for attention and interpretation reduces trait and state anxiety in anxious patients referred to an out-patient service: Results from a pilot. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 42(3), 258–64. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.12.006
Brown, C. C. M., Diekman, A. B. A., Tennial, R. E., & Solomon, E. D. (2011). Alone and happy: Personality moderates the effect of happy mood on social approach. Journal of Research in Personality, 45(6), 702–705. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2011.08.007
Brown, J. D. J., & Brown, M. A. (2011). Self-reflection and feelings of self-worth: When Rosenberg meets Heisenberg. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(6), 1269–1275. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2011.05.019
Brown, J. J. R., & Kocovski, N. N. L. (2013). Perfectionism as a Predictor of Post-event Rumination in a Socially Anxious Sample. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 32(2), 150–163. doi:10.1007/s10942-013-0175-y
Brunstein, J. J. C. (1993). Personal goals and subjective well-being: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(5), 1061–1070. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.65.5.1061
Burroughs, M. D. (2014). Sally Haslanger, “Resisting Reality: Social Construction and Social Critique.” Social Theory and Practice, 40(1), 145–152. http://doi.org/10.5840/soctheorpract20144017
Cahill, J. M., & Polich, J. (1992). P300, probability, and introverted/extroverted personality types. Biological Psychology, 33(1), 23–35. doi:10.1016/0301-0511(92)90003-D
Calhoun, L. G., Cann, A., Tedeschi, R. G., & McMillan, J. (2000). A correlational test of the relationship between posttraumatic growth, religion, and cognitive processing. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 13(3), 521–7. doi:10.1023/A:1007745627077
Campbell, J. B., & Heller, J. F. (1987). Correlations of extraversion, impulsivity and sociability with sensation seeking and MBTI-introversion. Personality and Individual Differences, 8(1), 133–136. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(87)90021-3
Cameron, P. (1975). Mood as an indicant of happiness: ags, sex,social class, and situational differences. Journal of Gerontology, 30, 216–224. doi:10.1093/geronj/30.2.216
Capik, C., & Gozum, S. (2014). Psychometric features of an assessment instrument with likert and dichotomous response formats. Public Health Nursing (Boston, Mass.), 32(1), 81–6. doi:10.1111/phn.12156
Capraro, R. M., & Capraro, M. M. (2002). Myers-Briggs Type Indicator Score Reliability Across: Studies a Meta-Analytic Reliability Generalization Study. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 62(4), 590–602. doi:10.1177/0013164402062004004
Caprara, G. V., Steca, P., Alessandri, G., Abela, J. R., & McWhinnie, C. M. (2011). Positive orientation: explorations on what is common to life satisfaction, self-esteem, and optimism. Epidemiologia E Psichiatria Sociale, 19(01), 63–71. doi:10.1017/S1121189X00001615
Carlson, R. (1980). Studies of Jungian typology: II. Representations of the personal world. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38(5), 801–810. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.38.5.801
Carlson, R., & Levy, N. (1973). Studies of Jungian typology: I. Memory, social perception, and social action1. Journal of Personality, 41(4), 559–576. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1973.tb00111.x
Carlyn, M. (1977). An assessment of the Myers-Briggs Type indicator. Journal of Personality Assessment, 41(5), 461–73. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4105_2
Carpentier, J., Mageau, G. G. A., & Vallerand, R. J. R. (2011). Ruminations and Flow: Why Do People with a More Harmonious Passion Experience Higher Well-Being? Journal of Happiness Studies, 13(3), 501–518. doi:10.1007/s10902-011-9276-4
Carrere, R. A. (2013). Review of Psychotherapy lives intersecting. Psychotherapy, 50(4), 593–594. doi:10.1037/a0032155
Carroll, K. K. M., Nich, C., Lapaglia, D. D. M., Peters, E. N., Easton, C. J., & Petry, N. M. (2012). Combining cognitive behavioral therapy and contingency management to enhance their effects in treating cannabis dependence: less can be more, more or less. Addiction (Abingdon, England), 107(9), 1650–9. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03877.x
Carthy, J., Dudley, A., & Ayres, J. (2013). Is CBT an appropriate intervention in reducing anger and aggression? British Journal of Mental Health Nursing, 2(5), 268–273. doi:10.12968/bjmh.2013.2.5.268
Castonguay, L. G. (2013). Psychotherapy outcome: an issue worth re-revisiting 50 years later. Psychotherapy (Chicago, Ill.), 50(1), 52–67. doi:10.1037/a0030898
Caunt, B. B. S. B., Franklin, J., Brodaty, N. E. N., & Brodaty, H. (2013). Exploring the causes of subjective well-being: A content analysis of peoples’ recipes for long-term happiness. Journal of Happiness Studies, 14(2), 475–499. doi:10.1007/s10902-012-9339-1
Chang, J., Zhang, M., Hitchman, G., Qiu, J., & Liu, Y. (2014). When you smile, you become happy: Evidence from resting state task-based fMRI. Biological Psychology, 103, 100 – 106. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.08.003
Chen, J., Rapee, R. R. M., & Abbott, M. J. M. (2013). Mediators of the relationship between social anxiety and post-event rumination. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 27(1), 1–8. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.10.008
Cheng, C. M. C., Govorun, O., & Chartrand, T. L. T. (2012). Effect of Self-awareness on Negative Affect Among Individuals with Discrepant Low Self-esteem. Self and Identity, 11(3), 304–316. doi:10.1080/15298868.2011.567022
Cienki, A. (2013). Image schemas and mimetic schemas in cognitive linguistics and gesture studies. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 11(2), 417–432. doi:10.1075/rcl.11.2.13cie
Ciraulo, D. A. DA, Barlow, D. H. D., Gulliver, S. B. S., Farchione, T., Morissette, S. B., Kamholz, B. W., Knapp, C. M. (2013). The effects of venlafaxine and cognitive behavioral therapy alone and combined in the treatment of co-morbid alcohol use-anxiety disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51(11), 729–35. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2013.08.003
Clark, D. M. D. (2011). Implementing NICE guidelines for the psychological treatment of depression and anxiety disorders: the IAPT experience. International Review of Psychiatry (Abingdon, England), 23(4), 318–27. doi:10.3109/09540261.2011.606803
Collard, P., Avny, N., & Boniwell, I. (2008). Teaching Mindfulness Based Cognitive Therapy (MBCT) to students: The effects of MBCT on the levels of Mindfulness and Subjective Well-Being. Counselling Psychology Quarterly, 21(4), 323–336. doi:10.1080/09515070802602112
Connors, M. E. (2011). Attachment theory: A “secure base” for psychotherapy integration. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 21(3), 348–362. doi:10.1037/a0025460
Cook-Greuter, S. R., & Soulen, J. (2007). The Developmental Perspective in Integral Counseling. Counseling and Values, 51(3), 180–192. doi:10.1002/j.2161-007X.2007.tb00077.x
Cook-Greuter, S. R. (2004). Making the case for a developmental perspective. Industrial and Commercial Training, 36(7), 275–281. doi:10.1108/00197850410563902
Cools, E., & Van den Broeck, H. (2007). Development and Validation of the Cognitive Style Indicator. The Journal of Psychology, 141(4), 359–387. doi:10.3200/JRLP.141.4.359-388
Costa, P. P. T. P., & McCrae, R. R. R. (1980). Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being: Happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38(4), 668–678. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.38.4.668
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Four ways five factors are basic. Personality and Individual Differences, 13(6), 653–665. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(92)90236-i
Costa, P. T., Zonderman, A. B., McCrae, R. R., Huntley, J. C., Locke, B. Z., & Barbano, H. E. (1987). Longitudinal analyses of psychological well-being in a national sample: Stability of mean levels. Journal of Gerontology, 42(1), 50-55.
Cotton, S., Luberto, C. M., Sears, R. W., Strawn, J. R., Stahl, L., Wasson, R. S., Delbello, M. P. (2015). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for youth with anxiety disorders at risk for bipolar disorder: a pilot trial. Early Intervention in Psychiatry. doi:10.1111/eip.12216
Cozijnsen, R., Stevens, N. L., & Tilburg, V. (2010). Maintaining work‐related personal ties following retirement. Personal Relationships, 17(3), 345-356.
Cozzolino, P. J., & Blackie, L. E. R. (2013). I Die, Therefore I Am: The Pursuit of Meaning in the Light of Death. The Experience of Meaning in Life, 31–45. doi:10.1007/978-94-007-6527-6_3
Cranney, S. (2013). Do People Who Believe in God Report More Meaning in Their Lives? The Existential Effects of Belief. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 52(3), 638–646. doi:10.1111/jssr.12046
Crawford, S. S. A., & Caltabiano, N. N. J. (2011). Promoting emotional well-being through the use of humour. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 6(3), 237–252. doi:10.1080/17439760.2011.577087
Crocker, J. (2011). The Paradoxical Consequences of Interpersonal Goals: Relationships, Distress, and the Self. Psychological Studies, 56(1), 142–150. doi:10.1007/s12646-011-0064-3
Cross, L. W. (2013). Psychodynamic therapy and motivational interviewing: Approaches to medical issues in low motivation anorexics. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 23(1), 28–38. doi:10.1037/a0030270
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow : the psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper & Row.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity : flow and the psychology of discovery and invention. New York: HarperCollinsPublishers.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Finding flow: the psychology of engagement with everyday life. Choice Reviews Online, 35(03), 35–1828–35–1828. doi:10.5860/CHOICE.35-1828
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2014a). Flow with Soul. Flow and the Foundations of Positive Psychology, 265–278. doi:10.1007/978-94-017-9088-8_17
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2014b). Play and Intrinsic Rewards. Flow and the Foundations of Positive Psychology, 135–153. doi:10.1007/978-94-017-9088-8_10
Cuijpers, P., Driessen, E., Hollon, S. S. D., van Oppen, P., Barth, J., & Andersson, G. (2012). The efficacy of non-directive supportive therapy for adult depression: a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 32(4), 280–91. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2012.01.003
Cunningham, M. R. (1988). Does happiness mean friendliness? Induced mood and heterosexual self-disclosure.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14(2), 283–297. doi:10.1177/0146167288142007
Daaleman, T. T. P., Cobb, A. K. A., & Frey, B. B. B. (2001). Spirituality and well-being: an exploratory study of the patient perspective. Social Science & Medicine, 53(11), 1503–1511. doi:10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00439-1
David, O. A., Matu, S. A. S., Pintea, S., Cotet, C. D., & Nagy, D. (2014). Cognitive-Behavioral Processes Based on Using the ABC Analysis by Trainees’ for Their Personal Development. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 32(3), 198–215. doi:10.1007/s10942-014-0189-0
Davidson, R., Pacek, A. C., & Radcliff, B. (2013). Public Policy and Human Happiness: The Welfare State and the Market as Agents of Well-Being. Happiness Studies Book Series, 163–175. doi:10.1007/978-94-007-6609-9_12
Deblinger, E., Mannarino, A. P. A., Cohen, J. A., Runyon, M. K., & Steer, R. A. (2011). Trauma-focused cognitive behavioral therapy for children: impact of the trauma narrative and treatment length. Depression and Anxiety, 28(1), 67–75. doi:10.1002/da.20744
Deffenbacher, J. J. L. (2011). Cognitive-Behavioral Conceptualization and Treatment of Anger. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 18(2), 212–221. doi:10.1016/j.cbpra.2009.12.004
DeMaris, A., Sanchez, L. A., & Krivickas, K. (2012). Developmental Patterns in Marital Satisfaction: Another Look at Covenant Marriage. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 74(5), 989–1004. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2012.00999.x
Dember, W. N., & Brooks, J. (1989). A new instrument for measuring optimism and pessimism: Test-retest reliability and relations with happiness and religious commitment. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 27(4), 365-366.
Deng, S., Liu, Y., Li, H., & Hu, F. (2013). How Does Personality Matter? An Investigation of the Impact of Extraversion on Individuals’ SNS Use. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 16(8), 575–81. doi:10.1089/cyber.2012.0383
Devine, P. G. P., Forscher, P. P. S., Austin, A. A. J., & Cox, W. W. T. L. (2012). Long-term reduction in implicit race bias: A prejudice habit-breaking intervention. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(6), 1267–1278. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2012.06.003
Dickter, C., & Gyurovski, I. (2012). The effects of expectancy violations on early attention to race in an impression-formation paradigm. Social Neuroscience, 7(3), 240–51. doi:10.1080/17470919.2011.609906
Diener, E. (1996). Traits Can Be Powerful, but Are Not Enough: Lessons from Subjective Well-Being. Journal of Research in Personality, 30(3), 389–399. doi:10.1006/jrpe.1996.0027
Diener, E. (2012). New findings and future directions for subjective well-being research. American Psychologist, 67(8), 590–7. doi:10.1037/a0029541
Diener, E. (2013). The remarkable changes in the science of subjective well-being. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(6), 663–666. doi:10.1177/1745691613507583
Diener, E., & Chan, M. Y. (2011). Happy people live longer: Subjective well‐being contributes to health and longevity. Applied Psychology: Health and Well‐Being, 3(1), 1-43.
Diener, E., & Diener, C. (2011). Monitoring psychosocial prosperity for social change. In Positive psychology as social change (pp. 53-71). Springer Netherlands.
Diener, E., Fujita, F., Tay, L., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2012). Purpose, mood, and pleasure in predicting satisfaction judgments. Social indicators research,105(3), 333-341.
Diener, E., Horwitz, J., & Emmons, R. A. (1985). Happiness of the very wealthy. Social Indicators Research, 16(3), 263–274. doi:10.1007/bf00415126
Diener, E., Inglehart, R., & Tay, L. (2012). Theory and Validity of Life Satisfaction Scales. Social Indicators Research, 112(3), 497–527. doi:10.1007/s11205-012-0076-y
Diener, E., Larsen, R. J., & Emmons, R. A. (1984). Person× Situation interactions: Choice of situations and congruence response models. Journal of personality and social psychology, 47(3), 580.
Diener, E., Lucas, R. E., & Scollon, C. N. (2006). Beyond the hedonic treadmill: revising the adaptation theory of well-being. American Psychologist, 61(4), 305.
Diener, E., Ng, W., Harter, J., & Arora, R. (2010). Wealth and happiness across the world: material prosperity predicts life evaluation, whereas psychosocial prosperity predicts positive feeling. Journal of personality and social psychology, 99(1), 52.
Diener, E., Nickerson, C., Lucas, R. E., & Sandvik, E. (2002). Dispositional Affect and Job Outcomes. Social Indicators Research, 59(3), 229–259. doi:10.1023/a:1019672513984
Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Ryan, K. L. (2013). Universals and cultural differences in the causes and structure of happiness: A multilevel review. In Mental well-being (pp. 153-176). Springer Netherlands.
Diener, E. S. R. E., Oishi, S., & Lucas, R. E. R. (2003). Personality, culture, and subjective well-being: emotional and cognitive evaluations of life. Annual Review of Psychology. 2003, 54(1), 403–25. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145056
Diener, E., & Scollon, C. N. C. (2014). The What, Why, When, and How of Teaching the Science of Subjective Well-Being. Teaching of Psychology, 41(2), 175–183. doi:10.1177/0098628314530346
Diener, E., & Tov, W. (2007). Subjective Well‐Being and Peace. Journal of Social Issues, 63(2), 421-440.
Diener, E., Emmons, R.A., Larson, R. ., Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction With Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(1), 71–5. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13
Dierendonck, D. V., & Mohan, K. (2006). Some thoughts on spirituality and eudaimonic well-being. Mental health, religion and culture, 9(03), 227-238.
DiGiuseppe, R. A., DiGiuseppe, R., Doyle, K. A., Dryden, W., & Backx, W. (2013). A Practitioner’s Guide to Rational-emotive Behavior Therapy. Oxford University Press.
Dijksterhuis, A., & Aarts, H. (2010). Goals, attention, and (un) consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology, 61(1), 467–90. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100445
Dolan, P., Peasgood, T., & White, M. (2008). Do we really know what makes us happy? A review of the economic literature on the factors associated with subjective well-being. Journal of economic psychology, 29(1), 94-122.
Dolphin, L., & Hennessy, E. (2014). Adolescents׳ perceptions of peers with depression: An attributional analysis. Psychiatry Research, 218(3), 295–302. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2014.04.051
Doyen, S., Klein, O., Simons, D. J., & Cleeremans, A. (2014). On the Other Side of the Mirror: Priming in Cognitive and Social Psychology. Social Cognition, 32(Supplement), 12–32. doi:10.1521/soco.2014.32.supp.12
Drisko, J. J. W., & Simmons, B. M. B. (2012). The Evidence Base for Psychodynamic Psychotherapy. Smith College Studies in Social Work, 82(4), 374–400. doi:10.1080/00377317.2012.717014
Dunkley, D. M., Berg, J. L., & Zuroff, D. C. (2012). The role of perfectionism in daily self‐esteem, attachment, and negative affect. Journal of personality, 80(3), 633-663.
Dush, C. M. K., & Amato, P. (2005). Consequences of relationship status and quality for subjective well-being. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 22(5), 607–627. doi:10.1177/0265407505056438
Duží, M., Jespersen, B., & Materna, P. (2010). Procedural Semantics for Hyperintensional Logic. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands. doi:10.1007/978-90-481-8812-3
Eastwood, J. D., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., & Smilek, D. (2012). The unengaged mind defining boredom in terms of attention. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(5), 482–495. doi:10.1177/1745691612456044
Ebert, J. P. J., & Wegner, D. M. D. (2011). Mistaking randomness for free will. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(3), 965–71. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2010.12.012
Edwards, J. A. J. (2003). The interactive effects of processing preference and motivation on information processing: Causal uncertainty and the MBTI in a persuasion context. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(2), 89–99. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00537-8
Edwards, J. A. J., Lanning, K., & Hooker, K. (2002). The MBTI and social information processing: An incremental validity study. Journal of Personality Assessment, 78(3), 432–50. doi:10.1207/S15327752JPA7803_04
Egan, S. S. J., Wade, T. T. D., & Shafran, R. (2011). Perfectionism as a transdiagnostic process: a clinical review. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(2), 203–12. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.009
Ehlers, A., Bisson, J., Clark, D. M. D., Creamer, M., Pilling, S., Richards, D.,Yule, W. (2010). Do all psychological treatments really work the same in posttraumatic stress disorder? Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 269–76. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.12.001
Ekman, P., Davidson, R. J., Ricard, M., & Wallace, B. A. (2005). Buddhist and psychological perspectives on emotions and well-being. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(2), 59-63.
el‐Guebaly, N., Mudry, T., Zohar, J., el-Guebaly, N., Tavares, H., & Potenza, M. N. (2012). Compulsive features in behavioural addictions: the case of pathological gambling. Addiction (Abingdon, England), 107(10), 1726–34. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03546.x
Elkins, D. N. (2012). Toward a common focus in psychotherapy research. Psychotherapy (Chicago, Ill.), 49(4), 450–4. doi:10.1037/a0027797
Ellis, A. (1991). The revised ABC’s of rational-emotive therapy (RET). Journal of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 9(3), 139–172. doi:10.1007/BF01061227
Ellis, A. (2001). Overcoming destructive beliefs, feelings, and behaviors: New directions for rational emotive behavior therapy. Prometheus Books.
Ellis, A. (2007). General semantics and rational-emotive therapy: 1991 Alfred Korzybski memorial lecture. ETC.: A Review of General Semantics.
Ellis, A. (2013). Better, Deeper And More Enduring Brief Therapy: The Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy Approach. Routledge.
Ellison, C. G. C., Fang, Q., Flannelly, K. J., & Steckler, R. A. (2013). Spiritual Struggles and Mental Health: Exploring the Moderating Effects of Religious Identity. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 23(3), 214–229. doi:10.1080/10508619.2012.759868
Elton, C. C. F., & Smart, J. J. C. (1988). Extrinsic job satisfaction and person-environment congruence. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 32(2), 226–238. doi:10.1016/0001-8791(88)90016-4
Emmons, R. R. A. (1991). Personal Strivings, Daily Life Events, and Psychological and Physical Well-Being. Journal of Personality, 59(3), 453–472. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1991.tb00256.x
Emmons, R. A., & Mishra, A. (2011). Why gratitude enhances well-being: What we know, what we need to know. Designing positive psychology: Taking stock and moving forward, 248-262.
Emmons, R. R. A., & Stern, R. (2013). Gratitude as a psychotherapeutic intervention. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 69(8), 846–55. doi:10.1002/jclp.22020
Enck, P., Junne, F., Klosterhalfen, S., Zipfel, S., & Martens, U. (2010). Therapy options in irritable bowel syndrome. European Journal of Gastroenterology & Hepatology, 22(12), 1402–11. doi:10.1097/MEG.0b013e3283405a17
Engberg, M., Alen, M., Kukkonen-Harjula, K., Engberg, E., Peltonen, J. E., Tikkanen, H. O., & Pekkarinen, H. (2012). Life events and change in leisure time physical activity. Sports Medicine (Auckland, N.Z.), 42(5), 433–47. doi:10.2165/11597610-000000000-00000
Ent, M. R. M., & Baumeister, R. F. R. (2014). Embodied free will beliefs: Some effects of physical states on metaphysical opinions. Consciousness and Cognition, 27, 147–54. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2014.05.001
Erdogan, B., Bauer, T. N. T., Truxillo, D. M., & Mansfield, L. R. (2012). Whistle While You Work A Review of the Life Satisfaction Literature. Journal of Management, 38(4), 1038–1083. doi:10.1177/0149206311429379
Evans, C., Richardson, J. T. E., & Waring, M. (2013). Field independence: reviewing the evidence. The British Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(Pt 2), 210–24. doi:10.1111/bjep.12015
Farach, F. J. F., Pruitt, L. L. D., Jun, J. J. J., Jerud, A. B., Zoellner, L. A., & Roy-Byrne, P. P. (2012). Pharmacological treatment of anxiety disorders: current treatments and future directions. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 26(8), 833–43. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.07.009
Fehm, L., Beesdo, K., Jacobi, F., & Fiedler, A. (2008). Social anxiety disorder above and below the diagnostic threshold: prevalence, comorbidity and impairment in the general population. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 43(4), 257–65. doi:10.1007/s00127-007-0299-4
Feldstein Ewing, S. W., & Chung, T. (2013). Neuroimaging mechanisms of change in psychotherapy for addictive behaviors: emerging translational approaches that bridge biology and behavior. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors : Journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors, 27(2), 329–35. doi:10.1037/a0031491
Ferreira, V. S., Slevc, L. R., & Rogers, E. S. (2005). How do speakers avoid ambiguous linguistic expressions?Cognition, 96(3), 263–84. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.09.002
Fintel, K. von, & Iatridou, S. (2003). Epistemic Containment. Linguistic Inquiry, 34(2), 173–198. doi:10.1162/002438903321663370
Fisk, G. G. M. (2010). “I want it all and I want it now!” An examination of the etiology, expression, and escalation of excessive employee entitlement. Human Resource Management Review, 20(2), 102–114. doi:10.1016/j.hrmr.2009.11.001
Fives, C. J., Kong, G., Fuller, J. R., & DiGiuseppe, R. (2011). Anger, aggression, and irrational beliefs in adolescents. Cognitive therapy and research, 35(3), 199-208.
Flanagan, C. M. C. (2010). The case for needs in psychotherapy. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 20(1), 1–36. doi:10.1037/a0018815
Flaxman, P. E., & Bond, F. W. (2010). Acceptance and commitment training: Promoting psychological flexibility in the workplace. Assessing mindfulness and acceptance processes in clients: Illuminating the theory and practice of change, 282-306.
Fordyce, M. M. W. (1988). A review of research on the happiness measures: A sixty second index of happiness and mental health. Social Indicators Research, 20(4), 355–381. doi:10.1007/BF00302333
Forgeard, M. J. C., Jayawickreme, E., Kern, M. L., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2011). Doing the Right Thing: Measuring Well-Being for Public Policy. International Journal of Wellbeing, 1(1). doi:10.5502/ijw.v1i1.15
Franz, C. C. E., Panizzon, M. S. M., Eaves, L. J. L., Thompson, W., Lyons, M. J., Jacobson, K. C., Kremen, W. S. (2012). Genetic and environmental multidimensionality of well-and ill-being in middle aged twin men. Behavior Genetics, 42(4), 579–91. doi:10.1007/s10519-012-9538-x
Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 359(1449), 1367–1377. doi:10.1098/rstb.2004.1512
Fredrickson, B. B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56(3), 218–226. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.218
Freeman, E. E. W. (2010). Therapeutic management of premenstrual syndrome. Expert Opinion on Pharmacotherapy, 11(17), 2879–89. doi:10.1517/14656566.2010.509344
Freeman, M. S., Hayes, B. G., Kuch, T. H., & Taub, G. (2007). Personality: A Predictor of Theoretical Orientation of Students Enrolled in a Counseling Theories Course. Counselor Education and Supervision, 46(4), 254–265. doi:10.1002/j.1556-6978.2007.tb00030.x
Friedrich, W. W., Cohen, D. D., & Wilturner, L. L. (1988). Specific beliefs as moderator variables in maternal coping with mental retardation. Children’s Health Care, 17(1), 40–44. doi:10.1207/s15326888chc1701_6
Frisch, M. B. (2013). Evidence-Based Well-Being/Positive Psychology Assessment and Intervention with Quality of Life Therapy and Coaching and the Quality of Life Inventory (QOLI). Social Indicators Research, 114(2), 193–227. doi:10.1007/s11205-012-0140-7
Frost, R. O., Glossner, K., & Maxner, S. (2010). Social anxiety disorder and its relationship to perfectionism. Social anxiety: Clinical, developmental, and social perspectives, 119-145.
Fujita, F., & Diener, E. (2005). Life satisfaction set point: stability and change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(1), 158–64. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.1.158
Fujita, F., Diener, E., & Sandvik, E. (1991). Gender differences in negative affect and well-being: The case for emotional intensity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(3), 427–434. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.61.3.427
Fujita, K. (2011). On conceptualizing self-control as more than the effortful inhibition of impulses. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15(4), 352–66. doi:10.1177/1088868311411165
Furnham, A. (1996). The big five versus the big four: the relationship between the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) and NEO-PI five factor model of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 21(2), 303–307. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(96)00033-5
Furnham, A., & Moutafi, J. (2012). Personality, age, and fluid intelligence. Australian Journal of Psychology, 64(3), 128–137. doi:10.1111/j.1742-9536.2011.00036.x
Furnham, A., Moutafi, J., & Crump, J. (2003). The relationship between the revised NEO-personality inventory and the Myers-Briggs type indicator. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 31(6), 577–584. doi:10.2224/sbp.2003.31.6.577
Gagné, M., & Deci, E. L. (2005). Self‐determination theory and work motivation. Journal of Organizational behavior, 26(4), 331-362.
Gámez, W., Chmielewski, M., Kotov, R., Ruggero, C., & Watson, D. (2011). Development of a measure of experiential avoidance: the Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Questionnaire. Psychological Assessment, 23(3), 692–713. doi:10.1037/a0023242
Garland, E. E. L., Fredrickson, B., Kring, A. M., Johnson, D. P., Meyer, P. S., & Penn, D. L. (2010). Upward spirals of positive emotions counter downward spirals of negativity: Insights from the broaden-and-build theory and affective neuroscience on the treatment of. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(7), 849–64. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.002
Garland, E. E. L., Gaylord, S. S. A., Palsson, O., Faurot, K., Douglas Mann, J., & Whitehead, W. E. (2012). Therapeutic mechanisms of a mindfulness-based treatment for IBS: effects on visceral sensitivity, catastrophizing, and affective processing of pain sensations. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 35(6), 591–602. doi:10.1007/s10865-011-9391-z
Gearing, R. R. E., Schwalbe, C. C. S. J., Lee, R., & Hoagwood, K. E. (2013). The effectiveness of booster sessions in CBT treatment for child and adolescent mood and anxiety disorders. Depression and Anxiety, 30(9), 800–8. doi:10.1002/da.22118
George, L. K., Ellison, C. G., & Larson, D. B. (2002). Explaining the relationships between religious involvement and health. Psychological inquiry, 13(3), 190-200.
Ghaderi, A. (2005). Psychometric Properties of the Self-Concept Questionnaire. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 21(2), 139–146. doi:10.1027/1015-5759.21.2.139
Gibson, W. (2011). Off the couch: Contemporary psychoanalytic approaches. Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy, 25(3), 298–301. doi:10.1080/02668734.2011.607249
Gold, J. (2011). Classics in the history of psychotherapy integration. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 21(2), 192–192. doi:10.1037/a0023955
Gore, W. L., & Widiger, T. A. (2013). The DSM-5 dimensional trait model and five-factor models of general personality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(3), 816–21. doi:10.1037/a0032822
Gottdiener, W. H. (2013). Assimilative dynamic addiction psychotherapy. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 23(1), 39–48. doi:10.1037/a0030271
Graham, J. R. J., Bradshaw, C., Surood, S., & Kline, T. J. B. (2014). Predicting Social Workers’ Subjective Well Being. Human Service Organizations Management, Leadership & Governance, 38(4), 405–417. doi:10.1080/23303131.2014.938584
Green, L. S., Oades, L. G., & Grant, A. M. (2006). Cognitive-behavioral, solution-focused life coaching: Enhancing goal striving, well-being, and hope. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 1(3), 142–149. doi:10.1080/17439760600619849
Greenberg, J. J. L., & Wilhelm, S. (2011). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for body dysmorphic disorder: A review and future directions. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 4(4), 349–362. doi:10.1521/ijct.2011.4.4.349
Grossman, P. (2010). Mindfulness for Psychologists: Paying Kind Attention to the Perceptible. Mindfulness, 1(2), 87–97. doi:10.1007/s12671-010-0012-7
Gruber, J., Harvey, A. G., & Gross, J. J. (2012). When trying is not enough: Emotion regulation and the effort–success gap in bipolar disorder. Emotion, 12(5), 997.
Grundmann, O., & Yoon, S. L. S. (2014). Complementary and alternative medicines in irritable bowel syndrome: An integrative view. World Journal of Gastroenterology : WJG, 20(2), 346–62. doi:10.3748/wjg.v20.i2.346
Hallion, L. L. S., & Ruscio, A. M. A. (2011). A meta-analysis of the effect of cognitive bias modification on anxiety and depression. Psychological Bulletin, 137(6), 940–58. doi:10.1037/a0024355
Hambrick, J. P., Turk, C. L., Heimberg, R. G., Schneier, F. R., & Liebowitz, M. R. (2003). The experience of disability and quality of life in social anxiety disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 18(1), 46–50. doi:10.1002/da.10110
Hampel, S., Weis, S., Hiller, W., & Witthöft, M. (2011). The relations between social anxiety and social intelligence: A latent variable analysis. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25(4), 545–53. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.01.001
Hampson, S. E. S. (2012). Personality processes: Mechanisms by which personality traits “get outside the skin.” Annual Review of Psychology, 63(1), 315–39. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100419
Hans, E., & Hiller, W. (2013a). A meta-analysis of nonrandomized effectiveness studies on outpatient cognitive behavioral therapy for adult anxiety disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 33(8), 954–64. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2013.07.003
Hans, E., & Hiller, W. (2013b). Effectiveness of and dropout from outpatient cognitive behavioral therapy for adult unipolar depression: a meta-analysis of nonrandomized effectiveness studies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 81(1), 75–88. doi:10.1037/a0031080
Harrington, N. (2005). It’s too difficult! Frustration intolerance beliefs and procrastination. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(5), 873–883. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.12.018
Hartwig, M. (2011). Bhaskar’s Critique of the Philosophical Discourse of Modernity. Journal of Critical Realism, 10(4), 485–510. doi:10.1558/jcr.v10i4.485
Harvey, C. D. H. C., Barnes, G. G. E., & Greenwood, L. (1987). Correlates of morale among Canadian widowed persons. Social Psychiatry, 22(2), 65–72. doi:10.1007/BF00584008
Harvey, R. J., & Murry, W. D. (2010). Scoring the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator: Empirical Comparison of Preference Score Versus Latent-Trait Methods. Journal of Personality Assessment, 62(1), 116–129. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6201_11
Haskayne, D., Hirschfeld, R., & Larkin, M. (2014). The outcome of psychodynamic psychotherapies with individuals diagnosed with personality disorders: a systematic review. Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy, 28(2), 115–138. doi:10.1080/02668734.2014.888675
Hauck, B. (2011). Introduction to” Difficulties in Learning to Apply General Semantics”. ETC.: A Review of General Semantics, 68(2), 121.
Havitz, M. E., Kaczynski, A. T., & Mannell, R. C. (2013). Exploring relationships between enduring, situational, and Psychological involvement in the contexts of self-defined Leisure and non-leisure activities. Leisure Sciences, 35(1), 45–62. doi:10.1080/01490400.2013.739890
Hay, P. (2013). A systematic review of evidence for psychological treatments in eating disorders: 2005–2012. The International Journal of Eating Disorders, 46(5), 462–9. doi:10.1002/eat.22103
Hay, P. P. J., & Claudino, A. A. M. (2012). Clinical psychopharmacology of eating disorders: a research update. The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology / Official Scientific Journal of the Collegium Internationale Neuropsychopharmacologicum (CINP), 15(2), 209–22. doi:10.1017/S1461145711000460
Hayes, S. C., Villatte, M., Levin, M., & Hildebrandt, M. (2011). Open, aware, and active: contextual approaches as an emerging trend in the behavioral and cognitive therapies. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 7(1), 141–68. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032210-104449
Heimberg, R. G. (2002). A cognitive-behavioral model of social anxiety disorder: Update and extension. Biological Psychiatry, 51(1), 101–108. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01183-0
Heintzelman, S. S. J., & King, L. A. (2014). (The feeling of) meaning-as-information. Personality and Social Psychology Review : An Official Journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc, 18(2), 153–67. doi:10.1177/1088868313518487
Heintzelman, S. S. J., Trent, J., & King, L. A. (2013). Encounters with objective coherence and the experience of meaning in life. Psychological Science, 24(6), 991–8. doi:10.1177/0956797612465878
Hellén, K., & Sääksjärvi, M. (2011). Happy people manage better in adverse services. Int J Qual & Service Sciences,3(3), 319–336. doi:10.1108/17566691111182861
Helliwell, J. F. (2003). How’s life? Combining individual and national variables to explain subjective well-being.Economic Modelling, 20(2), 331-360.
Henderson, L. W., Knight, T., & Richardson, B. (2013). An exploration of the well-being benefits of hedonic and eudaimonic behaviour. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 8(4), 322–336. doi:10.1080/17439760.2013.803596
Herian, M. M. N., Tay, L., Hamm, J. A. J., & Diener, E. (2014). Social capital, ideology, and health in the United States. Social Science & Medicine, 105, 30–7. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.01.003
Herrmann, M., & Brandstätter, V. (2013). Overcoming action crises in personal goals–Longitudinal evidence on a mediating mechanism between action orientation and well-being. Journal of Research in Personality, 47(6), 881–893. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2013.09.005
Hertel, P. T., & Brozovich, F. (2010). Cognitive habits and memory distortions in anxiety and depression. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(3), 155–160. doi:10.1177/0963721410370137
Hertel, P. T., & Mathews, A. (2011). Cognitive Bias Modification: Past Perspectives, Current Findings, and Future Applications. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(6), 521–536. doi:10.1177/1745691611421205
Hicks, J. J. A., Schlegel, R. J. R., & King, L. A. (2010). Social threats, happiness, and the dynamics of meaning in life judgments. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(10), 1305–17. doi:10.1177/0146167210381650
Hicks, J. J. A., Trent, J., Davis, W. E. W., & King, L. A. (2012). Positive affect, meaning in life, and future time perspective: an application of socioemotional selectivity theory. Psychology and Aging, 27(1), 181–9. doi:10.1037/a0023965
Higgins, E. T. E., & Eitam, B. (2014). Priming…Shmiming: It’s About Knowing When and Why Stimulated Memory Representations Become Active. Social Cognition, 32(Supplement), 225–242. doi:10.1521/soco.2014.32.supp.225
Hilbert, A., Bishop, M. M. E., Stein, R. I., Tanofsky-Kraff, M., Swenson, A. K., Welch, R. R., & Wilfley, D. E. (2012). Long-term efficacy of psychological treatments for binge eating disorder. The British Journal of Psychiatry : The Journal of Mental Science, 200(3), 232–7. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.110.089664
Hinshelwood, R. D. (2012). Group psychotherapy and mental health: Whose dynamics? European Journal of Psychotherapy & Counselling, 14(2), 201–212. doi:10.1080/13642537.2012.694687
Hobson, R. P. (2012). On the nature and standing of psychoanalytic psychotherapy. Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy, 26(3), 179–198. doi:10.1080/02668734.2012.709535
Hofmann, S. S. G., Asnaani, A., Vonk, I. J. J. I., Sawyer, A. T., & Fang, A. (2012). The efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy: a review of meta-analyses. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36(5), 427–440. doi:10.1007/s10608-012-9476-1
Holmes, E. A. E., & Mathews, A. (2010). Mental imagery in emotion and emotional disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(3), 349–62. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.01.001
Hoppitt, L., Mathews, A., Yiend, J., & Mackintosh, B. (2010). Cognitive bias modification: The critical role of active training in modifying emotional responses. Behavior Therapy, 41(1), 73–81. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2009.01.002
Horgan, J. (1994). Can Science Explain Consciousness? Scientific American, 271(1), 88–94. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0794-88
Hoshino, Y. (1991). A bias in favor of the positive response to high-frequency words in recognition memory. Memory & Cognition, 19(6), 607–616. doi:10.3758/BF03197156
House, J. S., Umberson, D., & Landis, K. R. K. (1988). Structures and Processes of Social Support. Annual Review of Sociology, 14(1), 293–318. doi:10.1146/annurev.so.14.080188.001453
Howell, J. L., & Shepperd, J. A. J. (2011). Demonstrating the Correspondence Bias. Teaching of Psychology, 38(4), 243–246. doi:10.1177/0098628311421320
Huang, G.-B. G., Wang, D. H. D., & Lan, Y. (2011). Extreme learning machines: a survey. International Journal of Machine Learning and Cybernetics, 2(2), 107–122. doi:10.1007/s13042-011-0019-y
Huang, J. J. Y., & Bargh, J. J. A. (2014). The Selfish Goal: Autonomously operating motivational structures as the proximate cause of human judgment and behavior. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37(2), 121–35. doi:10.1017/S0140525X13000290
Hyde, A. A. L., Maher, J. P. J., & Elavsky, S. (2013). Enhancing our understanding of physical activity and wellbeing with a lifespan perspective. International Journal of Wellbeing, 3(1), 98–115. doi:10.5502/ijw.v3i1.6
Iacovino, J. M. J., Gredysa, D. D. M., Altman, M., & Wilfley, D. E. (2012). Psychological treatments for binge eating disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports, 14(4), 432–46. doi:10.1007/s11920-012-0277-8
Ingersoll, R. E., & Cook-Greuter, S. R. (2007). The Self-System in Integral Counseling. Counseling and Values, 51(3), 193–208. doi:10.1002/j.2161-007X.2007.tb00078.x
Isen, A. A. M., Daubman, K. A. K., & Nowicki, G. G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 1122–1131. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.52.6.1122
Jacob, P. (1995). Can Semantic Properties Be Non-Causal? Philosophical Issues, 6, 44. doi:10.2307/1523028
Jacobs Bao, K., Lyubomirsky, S., & Bao, K. J. (2013). Making it last: Combating hedonic adaptation in romantic relationships. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 8(3), 196–206. doi:10.1080/17439760.2013.777765
Jain, M., & Sudhir, P. M. P. (2010). Dimensions of perfectionism and perfectionistic self-presentation in social phobia. Asian Journal of Psychiatry, 3(4), 216–21. doi:10.1016/j.ajp.2010.08.006
Janoff-Bulman, R. (1989). The Benefits of Illusions, the Threat of Disillusionment, and the Limitations of Inaccuracy. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 8(2), 158–175. doi:10.1521/jscp.1989.8.2.158
Jauhar, S., McKenna, P. J., Radua, J., Fung, E., Salvador, R., & Laws, K. R. (2014). Cognitive-behavioural therapy for the symptoms of schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis with examination of potential bias. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 204(1), 20–29. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.112.116285
Johnston, B. R. B., Colson, E., Falk, D., St John, G., Bodley, J. H., McCay, B. J., Slyomovics, S. (2012). On Happiness. American Anthropologist, 114(1), 6–18. doi:10.1111/j.1548-1433.2011.01393.x
Jordan, K. D. K., Masters, K. K. S., Hooker, S. A., Ruiz, J. M., & Smith, T. W. (2013). An Interpersonal Approach to Religiousness and Spirituality: Implications for Health and Well‐Being. Journal of Personality, 82(5), 418–31. doi:10.1111/jopy.12072
Judge, T. T. A., & Larsen, R. R. J. (2001). Dispositional Affect and Job Satisfaction: A Review and Theoretical Extension. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 86(1), 67–98. doi:10.1006/obhd.2001.2973
Judge, T. T. A., Piccolo, R. F. R., Podsakoff, N. P. N., Shaw, J. C., & Rich, B. L. (2010). The relationship between pay and job satisfaction: A meta-analysis of the literature. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 77(2), 157–167. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2010.04.002
Jung, C. G. (2012). Psychology of the Unconscious. Courier Dover Publications.
Jung, C. G. (2014). Collected Works of CG Jung, Volume 11: Psychology and Religion: West and East (Vol. 11). Princeton University Press.
Jung, C. (1971). The collected works of C.G. Jung. London: Routledge & Kegan.
Jung, C. (1983). The essential Jung. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press.
Jung, C. G. (2012). Psychology of the Unconscious. Courier Dover Publications.
Jung, C. G., & Campbell, J. (1971). The portable Jung (Vol. 70). Penguin Group USA.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness‐based interventions in context: past, present, and future. Clinical psychology: Science and practice, 10(2), 144-156.
Kaczmarek, L. D. L., Kashdan, T. B. T., Kleiman, E. M., Baczkowski, B., Enko, J., Siebers, A., Baran, B. (2013). Who self-initiates gratitude interventions in daily life? An examination of intentions, curiosity, depressive symptoms, and life satisfaction. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(7), 805–810. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.06.013
Kahneman, D., & Krueger, A. A. B. (2006). Developments in the Measurement of Subjective Well-Being. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 20(1), 3–24. doi:10.1257/089533006776526030
Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. A. B., Schkade, D., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. A. (2006). Would you be happier if you were richer? A focusing illusion. Science, 312(5782), 1908–10. doi:10.1126/science.1129688
Kalawski, J. P. (2011). Using Alba EmotingTM to work with emotions in psychotherapy. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 20(2), 180–7. doi:10.1002/cpp.790
Kandler, C., Bleidorn, W., Riemann, R., Angleitner, A., & Spinath, F. M. (2011). The Genetic Links Between the Big Five Personality Traits and General Interest Domains. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(12), 1633–1643. doi:10.1177/0146167211414275
Kaplan, M. A., & Hamati-Ataya, I. (2013). Transcending Postmodernism. (P. A. Heelan, Ed.). Palgrave Macmillan. doi:10.1057/9781137358578
Karn, J. S., Syed-Abdullah, S., Cowling, A. J., & Holcombe, M. (2007). A study into the effects of personality type and methodology on cohesion in software engineering teams. Behaviour & Information Technology, 26(2), 99–111. doi:10.1080/01449290500102110
Kashdan, T. T. B., DeWall, C. N., Pond, R. S., Silvia, P. J., Lambert, N. M., Fincham, F. D., Keller, P. S. (2013). Curiosity protects against interpersonal aggression: cross-sectional, daily process, and behavioral evidence. Journal of Personality, 81(1), 87–102. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2012.00783.x
Kashdan, T. T. B., Weeks, J. J. W., & Savostyanova, A. A. (2011). Whether, how, and when social anxiety shapes positive experiences and events: A self-regulatory framework and treatment implications. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(5), 786–99. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2011.03.012
Kassam, K. S. K., Morewedge, C. C. K., Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2011). Winners love winning and losers love money. Psychological Science, 22(5), 602–6. doi:10.1177/0956797611405681
Kasser, T., Rosenblum, K. L., Sameroff, A. J., Deci, E. L., Niemiec, C. P., Ryan, R. M., … Hawks, S. (2014). Changes in materialism, changes in psychological well-being: Evidence from three longitudinal studies and an intervention experiment. Motiv Emot, 38(1), 1–22. doi:10.1007/s11031-013-9371-4
Kelly, K. R., & Jugovic, H. (2001). Concurrent Validity of the Online Version of the Keirsey Temperament Sorter II. Journal of Career Assessment, 9(1), 49–59. doi:10.1177/106907270100900104
Kemp, E., & Kopp, S. S. W. (2011). Emotion regulation consumption: When feeling better is the aim. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 10(1), 1–7. doi:10.1002/cb.341
Kemppainen, J. J. K., MacKain, S., & Reyes, D. (2013). Anxiety symptoms in HIV-infected individuals. The Journal of the Association of Nurses in AIDS Care : JANAC, 24(1 Suppl), S29–39. doi:10.1016/j.jana.2012.08.011
Khoo, A., Dent, M. M. T., & Oei, T. P. S. T. (2011). Group cognitive behaviour therapy for military service-related post-traumatic stress disorder: Effectiveness, sustainability and repeatability. The Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 45(8), 663–72. doi:10.3109/00048674.2011.590464
Kier, F. J., Melancon, J. G., & Thompson, B. (1998). Reliability and Validity of Scores on the Personal Preferences Self-Description Questionnaire (PPSDQ). Educational and Psychological Measurement, 58(4), 612–622. doi:10.1177/0013164498058004005
Kihlstrom, J. J. F. (2014). Hypnosis and cognition. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1(2), 139–152. doi:10.1037/cns0000014
Kiko, S., Stevens, S., Mall, A. A. K., Steil, R., Bohus, M., & Hermann, C. (2012). Predicting post-event processing in social anxiety disorder following two prototypical social situations: State variables and dispositional determinants.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50(10), 617–26. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2012.06.001
Kim, J., Seto, E., Davis, W. E., & Hicks, J. A. (2014). Positive and Existential Psychological Approaches to the Experience of Meaning in Life. Meaning in Positive and Existential Psychology, 221–233. doi:10.1007/978-1-4939-0308-5_13
King, A. P., Erickson, T. M., Giardino, N. D., Favorite, T., Rauch, S. A. M., Robinson, E., Liberzon, I. (2013). A pilot study of group mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) for combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Depression and Anxiety, 30(7), 638–45. doi:10.1002/da.22104
Kleinstäuber, M., Witthöft, M., & Hiller, W. (2012). Cognitive-behavioral and pharmacological interventions for premenstrual syndrome or premenstrual dysphoric disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 19(3), 308–19. doi:10.1007/s10880-012-9299-y
Koerner, N., Antony, M. M., Young, L., & McCabe, R. E. (2013). Changes in Beliefs about the Social Competence of Self and Others Following Group Cognitive-Behavioral Treatment. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 37(2), 256–265. doi:10.1007/s10608-012-9472-5
Kohl, A., Rief, W., & Glombiewski, J. A. J. (2014). Do fibromyalgia patients benefit from cognitive restructuring and acceptance? An experimental study. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 45(4), 467–74. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.06.006
Korzybski, A. (1921). Manhood of humanity : the science and art of human engineering. New York: E.P. Dutton & Co.
Korzybski, A. (1994). Science and sanity : an introduction to non-Aristotelian systems and general semantics. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Institute of General Semantics.
Koster, E. H. W., De Lissnyder, E., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2011). Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: The impaired disengagement hypothesis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(1), 138–145. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.08.005
Krämer, S. (2013). Semantic values in higher-order semantics. Philosophical Studies, 168(3), 709–724. doi:10.1007/s11098-013-0157-z
Kristjansson, K. (2012). Selfhood, morality, and the Five-Factor Model of personality. Theory & Psychology, 22(5), 591–606. doi:10.1177/0959354311431193
Kross, E., Duckworth, A., Ayduk, O., Tsukayama, E., & Mischel, W. (2011). The effect of self-distancing on adaptive versus maladaptive self-reflection in children. Emotion, 11(5), 1032–9. doi:10.1037/a0021787
Kuo, J. R., Edge, I. G., Ramel, W., Edge, M. D., Drabant, E. M., Dayton, W. M., & Gross, J. J. (2011). Trait Rumination is Associated with Enhanced Recollection of Negative Words. Cognitive therapy and research, 36(6), 722–730. doi:10.1007/s10608-011-9430-7
Kuppens, P., & Tong, E. E. M. W. (2010). An appraisal account of individual differences in emotional experience.Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(12), 1138–1150. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00324.x
Kurdal, E., Tanriverdi, D., Savas, H. A., Tanrıverdi, D., & Savaş, H. (2013). The Effect of Psychoeducation on the Functioning Level of Patients With Bipolar Disorder. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 36(3), 312–28. doi:10.1177/0193945913504038
Labroo, A. A. A., Mukhopadhyay, A., & Dong, P. (2014). Not always the best medicine: Why frequent smiling can reduce wellbeing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 53, 156–162. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2014.03.001
Lambert, M. J. (2013). Outcome in psychotherapy: the past and important advances. Psychotherapy (Chicago, Ill.), 50(1), 42–51. doi:10.1037/a0030682
Lamers, S. M. A., Westerhof, G. G. J., Kovács, V., & Bohlmeijer, E. T. (2012). Differential relationships in the association of the Big Five personality traits with positive mental health and psychopathology. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(5), 517–524. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2012.05.012
Landor, M. (2013). Building happiness, resilience and motivation in adolescents: a positive psychology curriculum for well-being. Educational Psychology in Practice, 29(3), 329–330. doi:10.1080/02667363.2013.819667
Larrañaga, P., & Moral, S. (2011). Probabilistic graphical models in artificial intelligence. Applied Soft Computing, 11(2), 1511–1528. doi:10.1016/j.asoc.2008.01.003
Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1), 132–140. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.1.132
LaVeist, T. T. A., Thorpe, R. R. J., Pierre, G., Mance, G. A., & Williams, D. R. (2014). The relationships among vigilant coping style, race, and depression. The Journal of Social Issues, 70(2), 241–255. doi:10.1111/josi.12058
Layous, K., Chancellor, J., Lyubomirsky, S., Wang, L., & Doraiswamy, P. M. (2011). Delivering happiness: translating positive psychology intervention research for treating major and minor depressive disorders. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine (New York, N.Y.), 17(8), 675–83. doi:10.1089/acm.2011.0139
Layous, K., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2014). The How, Why, What, When, and Who of Happiness. Positive Emotion, 472–495. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199926725.003.0025
Layous, K., Katherine Nelson, S., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2012). What Is the Optimal Way to Deliver a Positive Activity Intervention? The Case of Writing About One’s Best Possible Selves. J Happiness Stud, 14(2), 635–654. doi:10.1007/s10902-012-9346-2
Leach, L. F. (2006). A Reliability Generalization Study of the Self-Description Questionnaire. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 66(2), 285–304. doi:10.1177/0013164405284030
Lee, G. R. G., & Bulanda, J. J. R. (2005). Change and consistency in the relation of marital status to personal happiness. Marriage & Family Review, 38(1), 69–84. doi:10.1300/J002v38n01_06
Lee, G. R. G., Seccombe, K., & Shehan, C. L. C. (1991). Marital status and personal happiness: An analysis of trend data. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53(4), 839. doi:10.2307/352991
Lee, K. K. S., & Ono, H. (2012). Marriage, Cohabitation, and Happiness: A Cross‐National Analysis of 27 Countries. Journal of Marriage and Family, 74(5), 953–972. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2012.01001.x
Lee-Flynn, S. C., Pomaki, G., DeLongis, A., Biesanz, J. C., & Puterman, E. (2011). Daily Cognitive Appraisals, Daily Affect, and Long-Term Depressive Symptoms: The Role of Self-Esteem and Self-Concept Clarity in the Stress Process. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(2), 255–268. doi:10.1177/0146167210394204
Lees, J. (2013). Psychotherapy, complementary and alternative medicine and social dysfunction. European Journal of Psychotherapy & Counselling, 15(3), 201–213. doi:10.1080/13642537.2013.810658
Leonard, N. H., Beauvais, L. L., & Scholl, R. W. (1995). A self concept-based model of work motivation. Academy of Management Proceedings, 1995(1), 322–326. doi:10.5465/AMBPP.1995.17536607
Leonard, N. H., Scholl, R. W., & Kowalski, K. B. (1999). Information processing style and decision making. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20(3), 407–420. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(199905)20:3<407::AID-JOB891>3.3.CO;2-V
Leonardi, G. (2012). The study of language and conversation with recurrence analysis methods. Psychology of Language and Communication, 16(2), 165-183.
Levinson, M. H. (2010). Alfred Korzybski and Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy. ETC.: A Review of General Semantics, 67(1), 55.
Levitt, H. M., & Piazza-Bonin, E. (2011). Therapists’ and clients’ significant experiences underlying psychotherapy discourse. Psychotherapy Research : Journal of the Society for Psychotherapy Research, 21(1), 70–85. doi:10.1080/10503307.2010.518634
Li, M., Fitzgerald, P., & Rodin, G. (2012). Evidence-based treatment of depression in patients with cancer. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 30(11), 1187–96. doi:10.1200/JCO.2011.39.7372
Light, K. J., Joyce, P. R., & Frampton, C. M. A. (2009). Description and validation of the Affective Temperament Questionnaire. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 50(5), 477–84. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2007.07.008
Lipton, B. H. (2008). The biology of belief: Unleashing the power of consciousness, matter and miracles. Hay House, Inc.
Littman-Ovadia, H., & Nir, D. (2014). Looking forward to tomorrow: The buffering effect of a daily optimism intervention. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 9(2), 122–136. doi:10.1080/17439760.2013.853202
Livingstone, K. M. K., & Srivastava, S. (2012). Up-regulating positive emotions in everyday life: Strategies, individual differences, and associations with positive emotion and well-being. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(5), 504–516. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2012.05.009
Lloyd, J. B. J. (2007). Opposition from Christians to Myers–Briggs personality typing: an analysis and evaluation. Journal of Beliefs & Values, 28(2), 111–123. doi:10.1080/13617670701485672
Lloyd, J. B. J. (2012a). Psychological type and the religious quest for wisdom and maturity. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 15(9), 837–847. doi:10.1080/13674676.2012.685607
Lloyd, J. B. J. (2012b). The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator® and mainstream psychology: analysis and evaluation of an unresolved hostility. Journal of Beliefs & Values, 33(1), 23–34. doi:10.1080/13617672.2012.650028
Loersch, C., & Payne, B. K. B. (2014). Situated Inferences and the What, Who, and Where of Priming. Social Cognition, 32(Supplement), 137–151. doi:10.1521/soco.2014.32.supp.137
Lorr, M. (1991). An empirical evaluation of the MBTI typology. Personality and Individual Differences, 12(11), 1141–1145. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(91)90077-O
Lothmann, C., Holmes, E. A. E., Chan, S. W. Y., & Lau, J. Y. F. (2011). Cognitive bias modification training in adolescents: effects on interpretation biases and mood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 52(1), 24–32. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02286.x
Löwstedt, M., & Räisänen, C. (2014). Social identity in construction: enactments and outcomes. Construction Management and Economics, 32(11), 1093–1105. http://doi.org/10.1080/01446193.2014.956132
Lucas, R. E. R. (2005). Time does not heal all wounds a longitudinal study of reaction and adaptation to divorce.Psychological Science, 16(12), 945–50. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01642.x
Lucas, R. E. R., & Baird, B. B. M. (2004). Extraversion and emotional reactivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(3), 473–85. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.3.473
Lucas, R. R. E. (2007). Adaptation and the Set-Point Model of Subjective Well-Being: Does Happiness Change After Major Life Events? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(2), 75–79. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00479.x
Lum, T. Y. T., & Lightfoot, E. (2005). The Effects of Volunteering on the Physical and Mental Health of Older People. Research on Aging, 27(1), 31–55. doi:10.1177/0164027504271349
Lundh, L. G. L., & Öst, L. G. L. (2001). Attentional bias, self-consciousness and perfectionism in social phobia before and after cognitive-behaviour therapy. Scandinavian Journal of Behaviour Therapy, 30(1), 4–16. doi:10.1080/02845710117841
Lyddon, W. J., & Robin, W. (2002). Cognitive psychotherapy and postmodernism: emerging themes and challenges. Clinical advances in cognitive psychotherapy: theory and application, 189-208.
Lyubomirsky, S. (2001). Why are some people happier than others? The role of cognitive and motivational processes in well-being. American Psychologist, 56(3), 239–249. doi:10.1037//0003-066X.56.3.239
Lyubomirsky, S. (2011). Hedonic adaptation to positive and negative experiences. Oxford Handbook of Stress, Health, and Coping. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195375343.013.0011
Lyubomirsky, S., Boehm, J. K. J., Kasri, F., & Zehm, K. (2011). The cognitive and hedonic costs of dwelling on achievement-related negative experiences: implications for enduring happiness and unhappiness. Emotion, 11(5), 1152–67. doi:10.1037/a0025479
Lyubomirsky, S., Dickerhoof, R., Boehm, J. K. J., & Sheldon, K. K. M. (2011). Becoming happier takes both a will and a proper way: an experimental longitudinal intervention to boost well-being. Emotion, 11(2), 391–402. doi:10.1037/a0022575
Lyubomirsky, S., Kasri, F., & Zehm, K. (2003). Dysphoric rumination impairs concentration on academic tasks.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 309-330.
Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive affect: does happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin, 131(6), 803–55. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.803
Lyubomirsky, S., & Layous, K. (2013). How Do Simple Positive Activities Increase Well-Being? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(1), 57–62. doi:10.1177/0963721412469809
Lyubomirsky, S., & Lepper, H. S. (1999). A measure of subjective happiness: Preliminary reliability and construct validation. Social indicators research, 46(2), 137-155.
Lyubomirsky, S., & Ross, L. (1997). Hedonic consequences of social comparison: a contrast of happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(6), 1141–1157. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.73.6.1141
Lyubomirsky, S., & Ross, L. (1999). Changes in attractiveness of elected, rejected, and precluded alternatives: A comparison of happy and unhappy individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 988–1007. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.76.6.988
Lyubomirsky, S., Sheldon, K. M., & Schkade, D. (2005). Pursuing happiness: The architecture of sustainable change. Review of General Psychology, 9(2), 111–131. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.9.2.111
Lyubomirsky, S., & Tucker, K. (1998). Implications of individual differences in subjective happiness for perceiving, interpreting, and thinking about life events. Motivation and Emotion, 22(2), 155–186.
MacDonald, K. (1995). Evolution, the Five-Factor Model, and Levels of Personality. Journal of Personality, 63(3), 525–567. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1995.tb00505.x
Mackintosh, M.-A., Morland, L. A. LA, Frueh, B. C., Greene, C. J., & Rosen, C. S. (2014). Peeking into the black box: Mechanisms of action for anger management treatment. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 28(7), 687–95. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2014.07.001
MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (2012). Cognitive bias modification approaches to anxiety. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8(1), 189–217. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143052
Macrae, C. N. C., & Johnston, L. (1998). Help, I need somebody: Automatic action and inaction. Social Cognition, 16(4), 400–417. doi:10.1521/soco.1998.16.4.400
Mak, W. W. S., Ng, I. S. W., & Wong, C. C. Y. (2011). Resilience: enhancing well-being through the positive cognitive triad. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 58(4), 610–7. doi:10.1037/a0025195
Mancini, A. A. D., Bonanno, G. A. G., & Clark, A. E. A. (2011). Stepping off the hedonic treadmill. Journal of Individual Differences, 32(3), 144–152. doi:10.1027/1614-0001/a000047
Mansell, W., Clark, D. M. D., & Ehlers, A. (2003). Internal versus external attention in social anxiety: an investigation using a novel paradigm. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41(5), 555–572. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00029-3
Marquis, A., & Wilber, K. (2008). Unification beyond eclecticism and integration: Integral psychotherapy. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 18(3), 350–358. doi:10.1037/a0013560
Marsh, H. W., Ellis, L. A., Parada, R. H., Richards, G., & Heubeck, B. G. (2005). A short version of the Self Description Questionnaire II: operationalizing criteria for short-form evaluation with new applications of confirmatory factor analyses. Psychological Assessment, 17(1), 81–102. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.17.1.81
Martindale, B., & Smith, J. (2011). Psychosis: Psychodynamic work with families. Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy, 25(1), 75–91. doi:10.1080/02668734.2011.542938
Martínez, M. P., Miró, E., Sánchez, A. A. I., Díaz-Piedra, C., Cáliz, R., Vlaeyen, J. W. S., & Buela-Casal, G. (2013). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for insomnia and sleep hygiene in fibromyalgia: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 37(4), 683–97. doi:10.1007/s10865-013-9520-y
Masi, C. M., Chen, H. Y., Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). A Meta-Analysis of Interventions to Reduce Loneliness. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15(3), 219–266. doi:10.1177/1088868310377394
Mason, E. (2012). Energy psychology and psychotherapy: A study of the use of energy psychology in psychotherapy practice. Counselling and Psychotherapy Research, 12(3), 224–232. doi:10.1080/14733145.2012.657208
Mason, O., & Hargreaves, I. (2001). A qualitative study of mindfulness‐based cognitive therapy for depression. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 74(2), 197-212.
Massaro, D. W., & Cowan, N. (1993). Information processing models: Microscopes of the mind. Annual Review of Psychology, 44(1), 383-425.
McAdams, D. P. (1992). The Five-Factor Model In Personality: A Critical Appraisal. Journal of Personality, 60(2), 329–361. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00976.x
McConnell, A. A. R., Shoda, T. T. M., & Skulborstad, H. H. M. (2012). The self as a collection of multiple self-aspects: Structure, development, operation, and implications. Social Cognition, 30(4), 380–395. doi:10.1521/soco.2012.30.4.380
McCrae, R. R. (2013). Exploring trait assessment of samples, persons, and cultures. Journal of Personality Assessment, 95(6), 556–70. doi:10.1080/00223891.2013.821075
McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T. . J., de Lima, M. P., Simões, A., Ostendorf, F., Angleitner, A., … Marušić, I. (1999). Age differences in personality across the adult life span: Parallels in five cultures. Developmental Psychology, 35(2), 466–477. doi:10.1037//0012-1649.35.2.466
McCrae, R. R. R., Jr, P. C., & Costa, P. T. . J. (1997). Personality trait structure as a human universal. American Psychologist, 52(5), 509–516. doi:10.1037//0003-066X.52.5.509
McCullough, M. E. M., Emmons, R. A., & Tsang, J. A. (2002). The grateful disposition: a conceptual and empirical topography. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(1), 112–127. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.82.1.112
McCullough, M. E. M., Hoyt, W. W. T., Larson, D. B., Koenig, H. G., & Thoresen, C. (2000). Religious involvement and mortality: a meta-analytic review. Health Psychology, 19(3), 211–222. doi:10.1037//0278-6133.19.3.211
McGloshen, T. H., & O’Bryant, S. S. L. (1988). The psychological well-being of older, recent widows. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 12(1), 99–116. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1988.tb00930.x
McHugh, R. K. R., Reynolds, E. E. K., Leyro, T. M., & Otto, M. W. (2013). An Examination of the Association of Distress Intolerance and Emotion Regulation with Avoidance. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 37(2), 363–367. doi:10.1007/s10608-012-9463-6
McIntosh, D. N. D., Poulin, M. J. M., Silver, R. C., & Holman, E. A. (2011). The distinct roles of spirituality and religiosity in physical and mental health after collective trauma: a national longitudinal study of responses to the 9/11 attacks. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 34(6), 497–507. doi:10.1007/s10865-011-9331-y
McIntosh, R. R. C., Seay, J. J. S., Antoni, M. H., & Schneiderman, N. (2013). Cognitive vulnerability for depression in HIV. Journal of Affective Disorders, 150(3), 908–15. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2013.05.014
McKee-Ryan, F., Song, Z., Wanberg, C. R., & Kinicki, A. J. (2005). Psychological and physical well-being during unemployment: a meta-analytic study. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(1), 53–76. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.1.53
McLeod, J. (2011). Psychotherapy in everyday life. Counselling and Psychotherapy Research, 11(1), 81–82. doi:10.1080/14733145.2011.548162
McMahon, J. (2011). REBT: Past, Present, and Future. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 29(4), 228–238. doi:10.1007/s10942-011-0145-1
McManus, F., Muse, K., Surawy, C., Hackmann, A., & Williams, J. M. G. (2014). Relating Differently to Intrusive Images: the Impact of Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy (MBCT) on Intrusive Images in Patients with Severe Health Anxiety (Hypochondriasis). Mindfulness. doi:10.1007/s12671-014-0318-y
McRae, K., Gross, J. J. J., Weber, J., Robertson, E. R., Sokol-Hessner, P., Ray, R. D., Ochsner, K. N. (2012). The development of emotion regulation: an fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal in children, adolescents and young adults. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(1), 11–22. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr093
Meevissen, Y. M. C., Peters, M. L. M., & Alberts, H. H. J. E. M. (2011). Become more optimistic by imagining a best possible self: effects of a two week intervention. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 42(3), 371–8. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.02.012
Mehta, S., Orenczuk, S., Hansen, K. T., Aubut, J.-A. L., Hitzig, S. L., Legassic, M., & Teasell, R. W. (2011). An evidence-based review of the effectiveness of cognitive behavioral therapy for psychosocial issues post-spinal cord injury. Rehabilitation Psychology, 56(1), 15–25. doi:10.1037/a0022743
Merz, E. E. L., & Roesch, S. C. (2011). Modeling trait and state variation using multilevel factor analysis with PANAS daily diary data. Journal of Research in Personality, 45(1), 2–9. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.11.003
Messay, B., Dixon, L. L. J., & Rye, M. S. M. (2012). The relationship between Quest religious orientation, forgiveness, and mental health. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 15(3), 315–333. doi:10.1080/13674676.2011.574271
Messer, S. B. (2012). The real relationship in psychotherapy: The hidden foundation of change. Psychotherapy Research, 22(3), 363–365. doi:10.1080/10503307.2011.643254
Michl, L. L. C., McLaughlin, K. A., Shepherd, K., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2013). Rumination as a mechanism linking stressful life events to symptoms of depression and anxiety: longitudinal evidence in early adolescents and adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(2), 339–52. doi:10.1037/a0031994
Milevsky, A., & Levitt, M. J. M. (2004). Intrinsic and extrinsic religiosity in preadolescence and adolescence: Effect on psychological adjustment. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 7(4), 307–321. doi:10.1080/13674670410001702380
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive-affective system theory of personality: reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure. Psychological Review, 102(2), 246–268. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.102.2.246
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1999). Integrating dispositions and processing dynamics within a unified theory of personality: The cognitive-affective personality system. Handbook of personality: Theory and research, 2, 197-218.
Mitchell, J. J. H., Newall, C., Broeren, S., & Hudson, J. J. L. (2013). The role of perfectionism in cognitive behaviour therapy outcomes for clinically anxious children. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51(9), 547–54. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2013.05.015
Mobily, K. E. (2014). Walking among older adults. World Leisure Journal, 56(2), 130–140. doi:10.1080/16078055.2014.903725
Mobini, S., Reynolds, S., & Mackintosh, B. (2013). Clinical implications of cognitive bias modification for interpretative biases in social anxiety: An integrative literature review. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 37(1), 173–182. doi:10.1007/s10608-012-9445-8
Mogilner, C., Kamvar, S. D. S., & Aaker, J. (2010). The Shifting Meaning of Happiness. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2(4), 395–402. doi:10.1177/1948550610393987
Molden, D. D. C. (2014). Understanding Priming Effects in Social Psychology: What is “Social Priming” and How does it Occur? Social Cognition, 32(Supplement), 1–11. doi:10.1521/soco.2014.32.supp.1
Momtaz, Y. A., Ibrahim, R., Hamid, T. A., & Yahaya, N. (2010). Mediating Effects of Social and Personal Religiosity on the Psychological Well Being of Widowed Elderly People. OMEGA–Journal of Death and Dying, 61(2), 145–162. doi:10.2190/OM.61.2.d
Moore, K. M., & Martin, M. E. (2014). Using MBCT in a Chronic Pain Setting: A Qualitative Analysis of Participants’ Experiences. Mindfulness. doi:10.1007/s12671-014-0363-6
Moran, J. J. M., Jolly, E., & Mitchell, J. J. P. (2014). Spontaneous mentalizing predicts the fundamental attribution error. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(3), 569–76. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00513
Moree, B. N. B., Davis, Thompson E., T. D., & Davis, T. E. (2010). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for anxiety in children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders: Modification trends. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4(3), 346–354. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2009.10.015
Morf, C. C., & Horvath, S. (2010). Self-Regulation Processes and Their Signatures. Handbook of personality and self-regulation, 115-144.
Morrison, A. S. A., & Heimberg, R. G. R. (2013). Social anxiety and social anxiety disorder. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9(1), 249–74. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185631
Morsella, E., & Bargh, J. J. A. (2010). What is an output? Psychological Inquiry, 21(4), 354–370. doi:10.1080/1047840X.2010.524597
Moscovitch, D. A., & Hofmann, S. S. G. (2007). When ambiguity hurts: Social standards moderate self-appraisals in generalized social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(5), 1039–52. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.07.008
Murakami, K. (2014). Absolute and relative judgments in relation to strength of belief in good luck. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 42(7), 1105–1115. doi:10.2224/sbp.2014.42.7.1105
Murray, J., & Russ, S. (1981). Adaptive regression and types of cognitive flexibility. Journal of Personality Assessment, 45(1), 59–65. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4501_12
Muse, K., McManus, F., Hackmann, A., & Williams, M. M. (2010). Intrusive imagery in severe health anxiety: Prevalence, nature and links with memories and maintenance cycles. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(8), 792–8. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.05.008
Myers, D. G., & Diener, E. (1995). Who is happy?. Psychological science, 6(1), 10-19.
Myers, D., Eid, M., & Larsen, R. (2008). Religion and human flourishing. The science of subjective well-being, 323-346.
Myers, I. B. I. (1962). The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator: Manual (1962). doi:10.1037/14404-000
Myers, I., McCaulley, M., & Most, R. (1998). MBTI manual: A guide to the development and use of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator.
Myers, I. B., & Myers, P. B. (2010). Gifts differing: Understanding personality type. Nicholas Brealey Publishing.
Myrseth, H., Molde, H., Støylen, I. J., Johnsen, B. H., Holsten, F., & Pallesen, S. (2011). A pilot study of CBT versus escitalopram combined with CBT in the treatment of pathological gamblers. International Gambling Studies, 11(1), 121–141. doi:10.1080/14459795.2011.559647
Naragon-Gainey, K., Gallagher, M. W., & Brown, T. A. (2014). A longitudinal examination of psychosocial impairment across the anxiety disorders. Psychological Medicine, 44(8), 1691–700. doi:10.1017/S0033291713001967
Narasimhan, N., Bhaskar, K., & Prakhya, S. (2010). Existential Beliefs and Values. Journal of Business Ethics, 96(3), 369–382. doi:10.1007/s10551-010-0472-7
Naudé, W., Amorós, J. E., & Cristi, O. (2014). “Surfeiting, the appetite may sicken”: entrepreneurship and happiness. Small Business Economics, 42(3), 523-540.
Neff, K. (2003). Self-Compassion: An Alternative Conceptualization of a Healthy Attitude Toward Oneself. Self and Identity, 2(2), 85–101. doi:10.1080/15298860309032
Neff, K. K. D., & Pommier, E. (2013). The relationship between self-compassion and other-focused concern among college undergraduates, community adults, and practicing meditators. Self and Identity, 12(2), 160–176. doi:10.1080/15298868.2011.649546
Nes, R. R. B., Røysamb, E., Harris, J. R., Czajkowski, N., & Tambs, K. (2010). Mates and marriage matter: genetic and environmental influences on subjective wellbeing across marital status. Twin Research and Human Genetics : The Official Journal of the International Society for Twin Studies, 13(4), 312–21. doi:10.1375/twin.13.4.312
Neuringer, A., & Jensen, G. (2012). The Predictably Unpredictable Operant. Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews, 7, 55–84. doi:10.3819/ccbr.2012.70004
Newton, T. (2013). Empathy in psychotherapy: how therapists and clients understand each other. Counselling and Psychotherapy Research, 13(1), 81–82. doi:10.1080/14733145.2013.764687
Newton, A. T. A., & McIntosh, D. N. D. (2010). Specific religious beliefs in a cognitive appraisal model of stress and coping. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 20(1), 39–58. doi:10.1080/10508610903418129
Ng, W., & Diener, E. (2014). What matters to the rich and the poor? Subjective well-being, financial satisfaction, and postmaterialist needs across the world. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(2), 326–38. doi:10.1037/a0036856
Nickerson, C., Diener, E., & Schwarz, N. (2011). Positive affect and college success. Journal of Happiness Studies, 12(4), 717-746.
Nisbet, E. K., Zelenski, J. M., & Murphy, S. A. (2011). Happiness is in our nature: Exploring nature relatedness as a contributor to subjective well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 12(2), 303-322.
Nistor, A. A. (2011). Developments on the Happiness Issue: a Review of the Research on Subjective Well-being and Flow. Scientific Journal of Humanistic Studies, 3(4).
Niven, K., Totterdell, P., Holman, D., & Headley, T. (2012). Does regulating others’ feelings influence people’s own affective well-being? The Journal of Social Psychology, 152(2), 246–60. doi:10.1080/00224545.2011.599823
Nobel, R., Manassis, K., & Wilansky-Traynor, P. (2011). The Role of Perfectionism in Relation to an Intervention to Reduce Anxious and Depressive Symptoms in Children. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 30(2), 77–90. doi:10.1007/s10942-011-0133-5
Nolan, P. (2013). The relational field of body psychotherapy. Body, Movement and Dance in Psychotherapy, 9(1), 29–40. doi:10.1080/17432979.2013.866598
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2012). Emotion regulation and psychopathology: the role of gender. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8(1), 161–87. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143109
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Aldao, A. (2011). Gender and age differences in emotion regulation strategies and their relationship to depressive symptoms. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(6), 704–708. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.06.012
Norris, C. C. J., Larsen, J. J. T., Crawford, L. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2011). Better (or worse) for some than others: Individual differences in the positivity offset and negativity bias. Journal of Research in Personality, 45(1), 100–111. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.12.001
Norris, J. I., Lambert, N. N. M., DeWall, C., & Fincham, F. D. (2012). Can’t buy me love?: Anxious attachment and materialistic values. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(5), 666–669. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.05.009
Nosek, B. A. B., Hawkins, C. C. B., & Frazier, R. S. R. (2011). Implicit social cognition: from measures to mechanisms. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(4), 152–9. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.01.005
O’Brien, C. (2012). Sustainable Happiness and Well-Being: Future Directions for Positive Psychology. Psychology,03(12), 1196–1201. doi:10.4236/psych.2012.312A177
Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(5), 242–9. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010
Oishi, S., Diener, E., & Lucas, R. R. E. (2007). The Optimum Level of Well-Being: Can People Be Too Happy?Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(4), 346–360. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00048.x
Okajima, I., Komada, Y., & Inoue, Y. (2011). A meta‐analysis on the treatment effectiveness of cognitive behavioral therapy for primary insomnia. Sleep and Biological Rhythms, 9(1), 24–34. doi:10.1111/j.1479-8425.2010.00481.x
Okajima, I., Nakajima, S., Ochi, M., & Inoue, Y. (2014). Reducing dysfunctional beliefs about sleep does not significantly improve insomnia in cognitive behavioral therapy. PloS One, 9(7), e102565. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0102565
Okajima, I., Nakamura, M., Nishida, S., Usui, A., Hayashida, K.-I., Kanno, M., Inoue, Y. (2013). Cognitive behavioural therapy with behavioural analysis for pharmacological treatment-resistant chronic insomnia. Psychiatry Research, 210(2), 515–21. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2013.06.028
Olatunji, B. O. B., Cisler, J. M. J., & Deacon, B. B. J. (2010). Efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy for anxiety disorders: a review of meta-analytic findings. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 33(3), 557–77. doi:10.1016/j.psc.2010.04.002
Olatunji, B. O. B., Kauffman, B. B. Y., Meltzer, S., Davis, M. L. M., Smits, J. J. A. J., & Powers, M. B. M. (2013). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis of treatment outcome and moderators. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(1), 33–41. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2014.05.002
Olatunji, B. O. B., Kauffman, B. B. Y., Meltzer, S., Davis, M. L., Smits, J. A. J., & Powers, M. B. (2014). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for hypochondriasis/health anxiety: a meta-analysis of treatment outcome and moderators.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 58, 65–74. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2014.05.002
Olesen, M. H. M. (2011). General causality orientations are distinct from but related to dispositional traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(4), 460–465. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.015
Ong, J. C., Ulmer, C. S., & Manber, R. (2012). Improving sleep with mindfulness and acceptance: a metacognitive model of insomnia. Behaviour research and therapy, 50(11), 651-660.
Orbell, S., & Verplanken, B. (2010). The automatic component of habit in health behavior: habit as cue-contingent automaticity. Health Psychology, 29(4), 374–83. doi:10.1037/a0019596
Owen, J. (2013). Early career perspectives on psychotherapy research and practice: psychotherapist effects, multicultural orientation, and couple interventions. Psychotherapy (Chicago, Ill.), 50(4), 496–502. doi:10.1037/a0034617
Owens, M., & Derakshan, N. (2013). The effects of dysphoria and rumination on cognitive flexibility and task selection. Acta Psychologica, 142(3), 323–31. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.01.008
Owens, M., Koster, E. H. W., & Derakshan, N. (2012). Impaired filtering of irrelevant information in dysphoria: an ERP study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(7), 752–63. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr050
Owings, L. R. L., Thorpe, G. L., McMillan, E. S., Burrows, R. D., Sigmon, S. T., & Alley, D. C. (2013). Scaling Irrational Beliefs in the General Attitude and Belief Scale An Analysis Using Item Response Theory Methodology. SAGE Open, 3(2). doi:10.1177/2158244013484156
Padesky, C. A. C., & Beck, A. A. T. (2003). Science and philosophy: Comparison of cognitive therapy and rational emotive behavior therapy. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 17(3), 211–224. doi:10.1891/088983903780906048
Paris, J. (2013). How the history of psychotherapy interferes with integration. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 23(2), 99–106. doi:10.1037/a0031419
Park, N., Park, M., & Peterson, C. (2010). When is the Search for Meaning Related to Life Satisfaction? Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 2(1), 1–13. doi:10.1111/j.1758-0854.2009.01024.x
Pavot, W. (2013). The Validity and Utility of Global Measures of Subjective Well-Being. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 44(2). doi:10.2478/ppb-2013-0020
Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (1993). Review of the satisfaction with life scale. Psychological Assessment, 5(2), 164–172. doi:10.1037//1040-3590.5.2.164
Pavot, W., Diener, E., Colvin, C. R., & Sandvik, E. (1991). Further validation of the Satisfaction with Life Scale: evidence for the cross-method convergence of well-being measures. Journal of Personality Assessment, 57(1), 149–61. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa5701_17
Pavot, W., Diener, E., & Suh, E. (1998). The Temporal Satisfaction With Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 70(2), 340–354. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa7002_11
Pe, M. L. M., Koval, P., & Kuppens, P. (2013). Executive well-being: Updating of positive stimuli in working memory is associated with subjective well-being. Cognition, 126(2), 335–40. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.10.002
Penttinen, H., Wahlström, J., & Kuusinen, K. L. (2013). Self-descriptions of socially phobic persons in short-term group psychotherapy. European Journal of Psychotherapy & Counselling, 15(1), 76–91. doi:10.1080/13642537.2013.765133
Perkes, S., Bowman, J., & Penkala, S. (2013). Psychological therapies for the management of co-morbid depression following a spinal cord injury: A systematic review. Journal of Health Psychology. doi:10.1177/1359105313496445
Peterson, C., Park, N., & Seligman, M. E. P. M. (2005). Orientations to happiness and life satisfaction: The full life versus the empty life. Journal of Happiness Studies, 6(1), 25–41. doi:10.1007/s10902-004-1278-z
Pienkos, E., & Sass, L. A. (2012). Empathy and Otherness: Humanistic and Phenomenological Approaches to Psychotherapy of Severe Mental Illness. Pragmatic Case Studies in Psychotherapy, 8(1). doi:10.14713/pcsp.v8i1.1119
Pineles, S. L. S., & Mineka, S. (2005). Attentional biases to internal and external sources of potential threat in social anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(2), 314–8. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.114.2.314
Posten, A. C. A., Ockenfels, A., & Mussweiler, T. (2014). How activating cognitive content shapes trust: A subliminal priming study. Journal of Economic Psychology, 41, 12–19. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2013.04.002
Poulsen, S., Lunn, S., Daniel, S. S. I. F., Folke, S., Mathiesen, B. B., Katznelson, H., & Fairburn, C. G. (2014). A randomized controlled trial of psychoanalytic psychotherapy or cognitive-behavioral therapy for bulimia nervosa. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(1), 109–16. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12121511
Powers, M. B. M., Halpern, J. M. J., Ferenschak, M. P., Gillihan, S. J., & Foa, E. B. (2010). A meta-analytic review of prolonged exposure for posttraumatic stress disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(6), 635–41. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.007
Pratchett, L. C., Daly, K., Bierer, L. M., & Yehuda, R. (2011). New approaches to combining pharmacotherapy and psychotherapy for posttraumatic stress disorder. Expert Opinion on Pharmacotherapy, 12(15), 2339–54. doi:10.1517/14656566.2011.604030
Prazeres, A. M. A., Nascimento, A. L., & Fontenelle, L. F. (2013). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for body dysmorphic disorder: a review of its efficacy. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 9, 307–16. doi:10.2147/NDT.S41074
Pulver, C. A. C., & Kelly, K. R. (2008). Incremental validity of the Myers-Briggs type indicator in predicting academic major selection of undecided university students. Journal of Career Assessment, 16(4), 441–455. doi:10.1177/1069072708318902
Purvis, A., Howell, R. R. T., & Iyer, R. (2011). Exploring the role of personality in the relationship between maximization and well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(3), 370–375. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.10.023
Querstret, D., & Cropley, M. (2013). Assessing treatments used to reduce rumination and/or worry: A systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 33(8), 996–1009. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2013.08.004
Quevedo, R. M., Abella, M. C., Marrero Quevedo, R. J., & Carballeira Abella, M. (2011). Well-being and personality: Facet-level analyses. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(2), 206–211. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.030
Rabinovich, M., & Kacen, L. (2011). Wish and overgeneralization model for psychotherapy integration. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 21(1), 43–69. doi:10.1037/a0022910
Rapee, R. M. R., & Heimberg, R. R. G. (1997). A cognitive-behavioral model of anxiety in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(8), 741–756. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00022-3
Rapgay, L., & Bystrisky, A. (2009). Classical mindfulness. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1172(1), 148-162.
Rashid, T. (2014). Positive psychotherapy: A strength-based approach. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 1–16. doi:10.1080/17439760.2014.920411
Rathod, S., Phiri, P., & Kingdon, D. (2010). Cognitive behavioral therapy for schizophrenia. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 33(3), 527–36. doi:10.1016/j.psc.2010.04.009
Ried, L. D. (2014). Using Likert-type and ipsative/forced choice items in sequence to generate a preference. Research in Social & Administrative Pharmacy : RSAP, 10(4), 598–607. doi:10.1016/j.sapharm.2013.09.001
Riskind, J. H. J., Calvete, E., Gonzalez, Z., Orue, I., Kleiman, E. M., & Shahar, G. (2013). Direct and Indirect Effects of Looming Cognitive Style via Social Cognitions on Social Anxiety, Depression, and Hostility. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 6(1), 73–85. doi:10.1521/ijct.2013.6.1.73
Ritchie, T. D. T., Sedikides, C., Wildschut, T., Arndt, J., & Gidron, Y. (2011). Self-concept Clarity Mediates the Relation between Stress and Subjective Well-being. Self and Identity, 10(4), 493–508. doi:10.1080/15298868.2010.493066
Robinson, M. M. D., & Gordon, K. K. H. (2011). Personality dynamics: Insights from the personality social cognitive literature. Journal of Personality Assessment, 93(2), 161–76. doi:10.1080/00223891.2010.542534
Robitschek, C. (1998). Personal growth initiative: The construct and its measure. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development.
Robitschek, C., Ashton, M. M. W., Spering, C. C., Geiger, N., Byers, D., Schotts, G. C., & Thoen, M. A. (2012). Development and psychometric evaluation of the Personal Growth Initiative Scale-II. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 59(2), 274–87. doi:10.1037/a0027310
Robitschek, C., & Cook, S. S. W. (1999). The influence of personal growth initiative and coping styles on career exploration and vocational identity. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 54(1), 127–141. doi:10.1006/jvbe.1998.1650
Robitschek, C., & Kashubeck, S. (1999). A structural model of parental alcoholism, family functioning, and psychological health: The mediating effects of hardiness and personal growth orientation. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 46(2), 159–172. doi:10.1037//0022-0167.46.2.159
Rosmarin, D. D. H., Pirutinsky, S., Cohen, A. B., Galler, Y., & Krumrei, E. J. (2011). Grateful to God or just plain grateful? A comparison of religious and general gratitude. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 6(5), 389–396. doi:10.1080/17439760.2011.596557
Rouder, J. N., Lu, J., Speckman, P., Sun, D., & Jiang, Y. (2005). A hierarchical model for estimating response time distributions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(2), 195-223.
Roy-Byrne, P., Craske, M. M. G., Sullivan, G., Rose, R. D., Edlund, M. J., Lang, A. J.,Stein, M. B. (2010). Delivery of evidence-based treatment for multiple anxiety disorders in primary care: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA : The Journal of the American Medical Association, 303(19), 1921–8. doi:10.1001/jama.2010.608
Røysamb, E., Tambs, K., Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., Neale, M. C., & Harris, J. R. (2003). Happiness and health: environmental and genetic contributions to the relationship between subjective well-being, perceived health, and somatic illness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(6), 1136–46. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1136
Rusk, R. D., & Waters, L. (2014). A psycho-social system approach to well-being: Empirically deriving the Five Domains of Positive Functioning. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 10(2), 141–152. doi:10.1080/17439760.2014.920409
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. The American Psychologist, 55(1), 68–78. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68
Ryff, C. D., & Singer, B. (1996). Psychological well-being: Meaning, measurement, and implications for psychotherapy research. Psychotherapy and psychosomatics, 65(1), 14-23.
Safran, J. D. (2013). Windows into clients’ perspectives on psychotherapy. Psychotherapy (Chicago, Ill.), 50(4), 535–6. doi:10.1037/a0034498
Saggino, A., & Kline, P. (1996). The location of the Myers-Briggs type indicator in personality factor space. Personality and Individual Differences, 21(4), 591–597. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(96)00009-8
Salanova, M., Rodríguez-Sánchez, A. M., Schaufeli, W. B., & Cifre, E. (2013). Flowing together: a longitudinal study of collective efficacy and collective flow among workgroups. The Journal of Psychology, 148(4), 435–55. doi:10.1080/00223980.2013.806290
Salter, D. D. W., Evans, N. J. N., & Forney, D. D. S. (2006). A longitudinal study of learning style preferences on the Myers-Briggs type indicator and learning style inventory. Journal of College Student Development, 47(2), 173–184. doi:10.1353/csd.2006.0022
Samuel, D. B., South, S. C., & Griffin, S. A. (2015). Factorial invariance of the Five-Factor Model Rating Form across gender. Assessment, 22(1), 65–75. doi:10.1177/1073191114536772
Sarafino, E. P., & Graham, J. A. (2006). Development and psychometric evaluation of an instrument to assess reinforcer preferences: the preferred items and experiences questionnaire. Behavior Modification, 30(6), 835–47. doi:10.1177/0145445506291393
Saucier, G. (1994). Mini-markers: a brief version of Goldberg’s unipolar big-five markers. Journal of Personality Assessment, 63(3), 506–16. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6303_8
Schimmack, U., Krause, P., Wagner, G. G., & Schupp, J. (2009). Stability and change of well being: An experimentally enhanced latent state-trait-error analysis. Social Indicators Research, 95(1), 19–31. doi:10.1007/s11205-009-9443-8
Schlegel, R. J., Hicks, J. A., King, L. A., & Arndt, J. (2011). Feeling Like You Know Who You Are: Perceived True Self-Knowledge and Meaning in Life. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(6), 745–756. doi:10.1177/0146167211400424
Schlenker, B. B. R., & Weigold, M. M. F. (1990). Self-consciousness and self-presentation: Being autonomous versus appearing autonomous. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(4), 820–828. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.59.4.820
Schloss, H. H. M., & Haaga, D. D. A. F. (2011). Interrelating Behavioral Measures of Distress Tolerance with Self-Reported Experiential Avoidance. Journal of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior Therapy : RET, 29(1), 53–63. doi:10.1007/s10942-011-0127-3
Schmidt, N. N. B. (2012). Innovations in the treatment of anxiety psychopathology: Introduction. Behavior Therapy, 43(3), 465–7. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2012.03.003
Schoenemann, P. T. P., & Wang, W. W. S.Y. (1996). Evolutionary principles and the emergence of syntax. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19(04), 646. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00043466
Schöttle, D., Huber, C. G. C., Bock, T., & Meyer, T. D. (2011). Psychotherapy for bipolar disorder: a review of the most recent studies. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 24(6), 549–55. doi:10.1097/YCO.0b013e32834b7c5f
Schreurs, K. M. G., Veehof, M. M., Passade, L., & Vollenbroek-Hutten, M. M. R. (2011). Cognitive behavioural treatment for chronic fatigue syndrome in a rehabilitation setting: effectiveness and predictors of outcome. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49(12), 908–13. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2011.09.004
Schueller, S. M. (2011). To each his own well-being boosting intervention: using preference to guide selection. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 6(4), 300–313. doi:10.1080/17439760.2011.577092
Schueller, S. M. S., & Seligman, M. M. E. P. (2010). Pursuit of pleasure, engagement, and meaning: Relationships to subjective and objective measures of well-being. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5(4), 253–263. doi:10.1080/17439761003794130
Schwartz, B., Ward, A., Monterosso, J., Lyubomirsky, S., White, K., & Lehman, D. R. (2002). Maximizing versus satisficing: happiness is a matter of choice. Journal of personality and social psychology, 83(5), 1178.
Shafir, T., Taylor, S. S. F., Atkinson, A. P. A., Langenecker, S. A., & Zubieta, J.-K. (2013). Emotion regulation through execution, observation, and imagery of emotional movements. Brain and Cognition, 82(2), 219–27. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2013.03.001
Shanahan, S., Jones, J., & Thomas-Peter, B. (2011). Are you looking at me, or am I? Anger, aggression, shame and self-worth in violent individuals. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 29(2), 77-91.
Shariff, A. F. A., & Norenzayan, A. (2007). God is watching you priming God concepts increases prosocial behavior in an anonymous economic game. Psychological Science, 18(9), 803–9. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01983.x
Sharot, T., Fleming, S. S. M., Yu, X., Koster, R., & Dolan, R. J. (2012). Is choice-induced preference change long lasting? Psychological Science, 23(10), 1123–9. doi:10.1177/0956797612438733
Sheeran, P., Gollwitzer, P. M. P., & Bargh, J. J. A. (2013). Nonconscious processes and health. Health Psychology, 32(5), 460–73. doi:10.1037/a0029203
Sheldon, K. K. M. (2014). Becoming Oneself The Central Role of Self-Concordant Goal Selection. Personality and Social Psychology Review. doi:10.1177/1088868314538549
Sheldon, K., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2006). Achieving sustainable gains in happiness: Change your actions, not your circumstances. Journal of Happiness Studies, 7(1), 55–86. doi:10.1007/s10902-005-0868-8
Sheldon, K. M., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2012). The challenge of staying happier: testing the Hedonic Adaptation Prevention model. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(5), 670–80. doi:10.1177/0146167212436400
Sherman, R. R. A., Nave, C. S. C., & Funder, D. C. D. (2010). Situational similarity and personality predict behavioral consistency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(2), 330–43. doi:10.1037/a0019796
Siddique, S. (2012). Storymaking: In-between anthropological enquiry and Transactional Analysis Psychotherapy.European Journal of Psychotherapy & Counselling, 14(3), 249–259. doi:10.1080/13642537.2012.713184
Sil, S., & Kashikar-Zuck, S. (2013). Understanding why cognitive-behavioral therapy is an effective treatment for adolescents with juvenile fibromyalgia. International Journal of Clinical Rheumatology, 8(2), 213 – 219. doi:10.2217/IJR.13.3
Sillick, W. W. J., & Cathcart, S. (2014). The relationship between religious orientation and happiness: the mediating role of purpose in life. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 17(5), 494–507. doi:10.1080/13674676.2013.852165
Silvia, P. J. P., & Phillips, A. G. A. (2011). Evaluating self-reflection and insight as self-conscious traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(2), 234–237. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.035
Simonton, D. D. K. (2013). Creative thoughts as acts of free will: A two-stage formal integration. Review of General Psychology, 17(4), 374–383. doi:10.1037/a0032803
Simsek, O. O. F. (2013). Self-absorption paradox is not a paradox: Illuminating the dark side of self-reflection. International Journal of Psychology, 48(6), 1109–21. doi:10.1080/00207594.2013.778414
Slavin-Mulford, J. (2013). The dance of psychotherapy. Psychotherapy (Chicago, Ill.), 50(3), 419–23. doi:10.1037/a0032692
Sockol, L. E. LE, Epperson, C. N., & Barber, J. P. J. (2011). A meta-analysis of treatments for perinatal depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(5), 839–49. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2011.03.009
Song, F., Holland, R., Barton, G. G. R., Bachmann, M., Blyth, A., Maskrey, V., Brandon, T. H. (2012). Self-help materials for the prevention of smoking relapse: study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials, 13(1), 69. doi:10.1186/1745-6215-13-69
Specht, J., Egloff, B., & Schmukle, S. C. (2011). Stability and change of personality across the life course: The impact of age and major life events on mean-level and rank-order stability of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(4), 862–882. doi:10.1037/a0024950
Spies, G., Asmal, L., & Seedat, S. (2013). Cognitive-behavioural interventions for mood and anxiety disorders in HIV: a systematic review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 150(2), 171–80. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2013.04.018
Spurr, J. J. M., & Stopa, L. (2002). Self-focused attention in social phobia and social anxiety. Clinical Psychology Review, 22(7), 947–975. doi:10.1016/S0272-7358(02)00107-1
Sripada, C., Swain, J. D. J. E. J., & Ho, S. S. (2014). Automatic goals and conscious regulation in social cognitive affective neuroscience. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37(2), 156–7. doi:10.1017/S0140525X13002161
Staw, B. B. M., Sutton, R. R. I., & Pelled, L. L. H. (1994). Employee Positive Emotion and Favorable Outcomes at the Workplace. Organization Science, 5(1), 51–71. doi:10.1287/orsc.5.1.51
Steger, M. F., & Kashdan, T. B. (2013). The unbearable lightness of meaning: Well-being and unstable meaning in life. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 8(2), 103–115. doi:10.1080/17439760.2013.771208
Steger, M., & Kashdan, T. (2008). Understanding the search for meaning in life: Personality, cognitive style, and the dynamic between seeking and experiencing meaning. Journal of Personality, 76(2), 199–228. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00484.x
Steger, M. M. F., & Frazier, P. (2005). Meaning in Life: One Link in the Chain From Religiousness to Well-Being. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52(4), 574–582. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.52.4.574
Steger, M. M. F., Frazier, P., Oishi, S., & Kaler, M. (2006). The meaning in life questionnaire: Assessing the presence of and search for meaning in life. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53(1), 80–93. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.53.1.80
Steger, M. M. F., Oishi, S., & Kesebir, S. (2011). Is a life without meaning satisfying? The moderating role of the search for meaning in satisfaction with life judgments. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 6(3), 173–180. doi:10.1080/17439760.2011.569171
Stein, D., & Grant, A. A. M. (2014). Disentangling the Relationships Among Self-Reflection, Insight, and Subjective Well-Being: The Role of Dysfunctional Attitudes and Core Self-Evaluations. The Journal of Psychology, 148(5), 505–22. doi:10.1080/00223980.2013.810128
Steiner, A. A. R. W., Petkus, A. J. A., Nguyen, H., & Loebach Wetherell, J. (2013). Information processing bias and pharmacotherapy outcome in older adults with generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 27(6), 592–7. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.10.007
Stewart-Sicking, J. A. (2013). Cognitive Therapy and the Punctual Self: Using an Ascetical Framework to Critique Approaches to Psychotherapy. Pastoral Psychology, 64(1), 111–122. doi:10.1007/s11089-013-0588-7
Stillman, T. F. T., Fincham, F. D. F., Vohs, K. D. K., Lambert, N. M., & Phillips, C. A. (2012). The material and immaterial in conflict: Spirituality reduces conspicuous consumption. Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(1), 1–7. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2011.08.012
Stopa, L., & Clark, D. M. D. (2000). Social phobia and interpretation of social events. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38(3), 273–283. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00043-1
Surawy, C., McManus, F., Muse, K., & Williams, J. M. G. (2014). Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy (MBCT) for Health Anxiety (Hypochondriasis): Rationale, Implementation and Case Illustration. Mindfulness. doi:10.1007/s12671-013-0271-1
Sutin, A. A. R., Costa, P. P. T., Miech, R., & Eaton, W. W. (2009). Personality and Career Success: Concurrent and Longitudinal Relations. European Journal of Personality, 23(2), 71–84. doi:10.1002/per.704
Swift, H. H. J., Vauclair, C.-M., Abrams, D., Bratt, C., Marques, S., & Lima, M.-L. (2014). Revisiting the paradox of well-being: The importance of national context. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbu011
Szasz, P. P. L., Szentagotai, A., & Hofmann, S. S. G. (2011). The effect of emotion regulation strategies on anger. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49(2), 114–9. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.11.011
Szuhany, K. K. L., Kredlow, M. A. M., & Otto, M. M. W. (2014). Combination Psychological and Pharmacological Treatments for Panic Disorder. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 7(2), 122–135. doi:10.1521/ijct.2014.7.2.122
Tanaka, Y., Kanazawa, M., Fukudo, S., & Drossman, D. A. (2011). Biopsychosocial model of irritable bowel syndrome. Journal of Neurogastroenterology and Motility, 17(2), 131–9. doi:10.5056/jnm.2011.17.2.131
Tay, L., & Diener, E. (2011). Needs and subjective well-being around the world. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(2), 354–65. doi:10.1037/a0023779
Taylor, S., Abramowitz, J. S. J., & McKay, D. (2012). Non-adherence and non-response in the treatment of anxiety disorders. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 26(5), 583–9. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.02.010
Taylor, S. S. E., & Brown, J. D. J. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103(2), 193–210. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.103.2.193
Teachman, B. A. B., Joormann, J., Steinman, S. A., & Gotlib, I. H. (2012). Automaticity in anxiety disorders and major depressive disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 32(6), 575–603. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2012.06.004
Teismann, T., Michalak, J., Willutzki, U., & Schulte, D. (2012). Influence of rumination and distraction on the therapeutic process in cognitive-behavioral therapy for depression. Cognitive therapy and research, 36(1), 15-24.
Terrizzi, J. A., Shook, N. J., & Ventis, W. L. (2012). Religious conservatism: an evolutionarily evoked disease-avoidance strategy. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 2(2), 105–120. doi:10.1080/2153599x.2012.695514
Textor, M. (2009). Devitt on the Epistemic Authority of Linguistic Intuitions. Erkenntnis, 71(3), 395–405. doi:10.1007/s10670-009-9176-8
Thoen, M. A., & Robitschek, C. (2013). Intentional growth training: developing an intervention to increase personal growth initiative. Applied Psychology. Health and Well-Being, 5(2), 149–70. doi:10.1111/aphw.12001
Thomas, A., Buboltz, W. C., & Winkelspecht, C. S. (2004). Job Characteristics and Personality as Predictors of Job Satisfaction. Organizational Analysis, 12(2), 205–219. doi:10.1108/eb028993
Thomas, Y., Gray, M. A., & McGinty, S. (2012). An exploration of subjective wellbeing among people experiencing homelessness: a strengths-based approach. Social Work in Health Care, 51(9), 780–97. doi:10.1080/00981389.2012.686475
Timmermans, B., Schilbach, L., Pasquali, A., & Cleeremans, A. (2012). Higher order thoughts in action: consciousness as an unconscious re-description process. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1594), 1412-1423.
Tkach, C., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2006). How do people pursue happiness?: Relating personality, happiness-increasing strategies, and well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 7(2), 183–225. doi:10.1007/s10902-005-4754-1
Toates, F. (2006). A model of the hierarchy of behaviour, cognition, and consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 15(1), 75–118. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2005.04.008
Tovote, K., Fleer, J., Snippe, E., Bas, I. I. V, Links, T. P., Emmelkamp, P. M., Schroevers, M. J. (2013). Cognitive behavioral therapy and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depressive symptoms in patients with diabetes: design of a randomized controlled. BMC Psychology, 1(1), 17. doi:10.1186/2050-7283-1-17
Tran, T. T. B., Hertel, P. T. P., & Joormann, J. (2011). Cognitive bias modification: induced interpretive biases affect memory. Emotion, 11(1), 145–52. doi:10.1037/a0021754
Tran, T. T. B., Siemer, M., & Joormann, J. (2011). Implicit interpretation biases affect emotional vulnerability: A training study. Cognition & Emotion, 25(3), 546–58. doi:10.1080/02699931.2010.532393
Trull, T. J. (2012). The Five-Factor Model of personality disorder and DSM-5. Journal of Personality, 80(6), 1697–720. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2012.00771.x
Tryon, G. S. (2013). Psychotherapy reflections: what I seek to accomplish in psychotherapy sessions. Psychotherapy (Chicago, Ill.), 50(3), 371–5. doi:10.1037/a0032032
Tryon, G. S., & Winograd, G. (2011). Goal consensus and collaboration. Psychotherapy (Chicago, Ill.), 48(1), 50–7. doi:10.1037/a0022061
Tsai, W., & Lau, A. S. A. (2013). Cultural differences in emotion regulation during self-reflection on negative personal experiences. Cognition & Emotion, 27(3), 416–29. doi:10.1080/02699931.2012.715080
Tugade, M. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). Resilient individuals use positive emotions to bounce back from negative emotional experiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(2), 320–33. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.2.320
Tugade, M. M., Fredrickson, B. L., & Barrett, L. F. (2004). Psychological resilience and positive emotional granularity: examining the benefits of positive emotions on coping and health. Journal of Personality, 72(6), 1161–90. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00294.x
Turk, C. L., Heimberg, R. G., Luterek, J. A., Mennin, D. S., & Fresco, D. M. (2005). Emotion Dysregulation in Generalized Anxiety Disorder: A Comparison with Social Anxiety Disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29(1), 89–106. doi:10.1007/s10608-005-1651-1
Uglanova, E. A. E., & Staudinger, U. U. M. (2013). Zooming in on Life Events: Is Hedonic Adaptation Sensitive to the Temporal Distance from the Event? Social Indicators Research, 111(1), 265–286. doi:10.1007/s11205-012-0004-1
Uleman, J. J. S., Rim, S. S., Adil Saribay, S., & Kressel, L. M. (2012). Controversies, questions, and prospects for spontaneous social inferences. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 6(9), 657–673. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2012.00452.x
Vacha-Haase, T., Thompson, B., & Vacha-Haase, T. (2002). Alternative Ways of Measuring Counselees’ Jungian Psychological-Type Preferences. Journal of Counseling & Development, 80(2), 173–179. doi:10.1002/j.1556-6678.2002.tb00180.x
Van der Put, C. E., Asscher, J. J., Stams, G. J. J. M., van der Laan, P. H., Breuk, R., Jongman, E., & Doreleijers, T. (2013). Recidivism After Treatment in a Forensic Youth-Psychiatric Setting The Effect of Treatment Characteristics. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 57(9), 1120–39. doi:10.1177/0306624X12452389
Van Hiel, A., Onraet, E., & De Pauw, S. (2010). The Relationship Between Social-Cultural Attitudes and Behavioral Measures of Cognitive Style: A Meta-Analytic Integration of Studies. Journal of Personality, 78(6), 1765–1800. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00669.x
Van Holst, R. J., van den Brink, W., Veltman, D. J., & Goudriaan, A. E. (2010). Why gamblers fail to win: a review of cognitive and neuroimaging findings in pathological gambling. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(1), 87-107.
Vanderlinden, J., Adriaensen, A., Vancampfort, D., Pieters, G., Probst, M., & Vansteelandt, K. (2012). A cognitive-behavioral therapeutic program for patients with obesity and binge eating disorder: short-and long-term follow-up data of a prospective study. Behavior Modification, 36(5), 670–86. doi:10.1177/0145445512439313
Varasdi, K. (2010). A Higher Order Extensional Framework for the Progressive. Journal of Semantics, 27(2), 177–206. doi:10.1093/jos/ffq002
Varga, S. (2014). Cognition, Representations and Embodied Emotions: Investigating Cognitive Theory. Erkenntnis, 79(1), 165-190.
Vassilopoulos, S. P., Moberly, N. J., & Douratsou, K. M. (2012). Social anxiety and the interaction of imagery and interpretations in children: An experimental test of the combined cognitive biases hypothesis. Cognitive therapy and research, 36(5), 548-559.
Vella-Brodrick, D. A. (2012). Positive Psychology Interventions: Research Evidence, Practical Utility, and Future Steps. Mental Well-Being, 331–353. doi:10.1007/978-94-007-5195-8_15
Verde, M. F., Stone, L. K., Hatch, H. S., & Schnall, S. (2010). Distinguishing between attributional and mnemonic sources of familiarity: the case of positive emotion bias. Memory & Cognition, 38(2), 142–53. doi:10.3758/MC.38.2.142
Verhaeghen, P., Joormann, J., & Aikman, S. N. (2014). Creativity, mood, and the examined life: Self-reflective rumination boosts creativity, brooding breeds dysphoria. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8(2), 211–218. doi:10.1037/a0035594
Verme, P. (2011). Life satisfaction and income inequality. Review of Income and Wealth, 57(1), 111–127. doi:10.1111/j.1475-4991.2010.00420.x
Vernon, A. (2011). Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy: The Past, Present, and Future. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 29(4), 239–247. doi:10.1007/s10942-011-0147-z
Viana, A. A. G., & Gratz, K. K. L. (2012). The role of anxiety sensitivity, behavioral inhibition, and cognitive biases in anxiety symptoms: structural equation modeling of direct and indirect pathways. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 68(10), 1122–41. doi:10.1002/jclp.21890
Vispoel, W. P., & Kim, H. Y. (2014). Psychometric properties for the Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding: dichotomous versus polytomous conventional and IRT scoring. Psychological Assessment, 26(3), 878–91. doi:10.1037/a0036430
Vosgerau, G. (2010). Memory and content. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(3), 838–46. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2010.06.021
Wallace, B. A. (2001). Intersubjectivity in Indo-Tibetan Buddhism. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(5-7), 5-7.
Walker, B. R., & Jackson, C. J. (2014). How the Five Factor Model and revised Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory predict divergent thinking. Personality and Individual Differences, 57, 54–58. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.09.011
Wampold, B. E. (2012). Humanism as a common factor in psychotherapy. Psychotherapy (Chicago, Ill.), 49(4), 445–9. doi:10.1037/a0027113
Warner, R. R. M., & Vroman, K. K. G. (2011). Happiness Inducing Behaviors in Everyday Life: An Empirical Assessment of “The How of Happiness.” Journal of Happiness Studies, 12(6), 1063–1082. doi:10.1007/s10902-010-9245-3
Warr, P., Butcher, V., & Robertson, I. (2004). Activity and psychological well-being in older people. Aging & Mental Health, 8(2), 172–83. doi:10.1080/13607860410001649662
Wasylkiw, L., Fabrigar, L. L. R., Rainboth, S., Reid, A., & Steen, C. (2010). Neuroticism and the architecture of the self: exploring neuroticism as a moderator of the impact of ideal self-discrepancies on emotion. Journal of Personality, 78(2), 471–92. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00623.x
Waterman, A. S. (2007). On the importance of distinguishing hedonia and eudaimonia when contemplating the hedonic treadmill. The American Psychologist, 62(6), 612–3. doi:10.1037/0003-066X62.6.612
Waugh, C. E. C., & Koster, E. H. W. E. (2014). A resilience framework for promoting stable remission from depression. Clinical Psychology Review. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2014.05.004
Webb, S. C. (1964). An Analysis of the Scoring System of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 24(4), 765–781. doi:10.1177/001316446402400404
Webb, T. L. T., Miles, E., & Sheeran, P. (2012). Dealing with feeling: a meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation. Psychological Bulletin, 138(4), 775–808. doi:10.1037/a0027600
Wegner, D. M. D., Erber, R., & Zanakos, S. (1993). Ironic processes in the mental control of mood and mood-related thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(6), 1093–1104. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.65.6.1093
Wenzlaff, R. M., & Wegner, D. M. (2000). Thought suppression. Annual Review of Psychology, 51(1), 59–91. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.59
White, L. K. L., Suway, J. J. G., Pine, D. D. S., Bar-Haim, Y., & Fox, N. A. (2011). Cascading effects: the influence of attention bias to threat on the interpretation of ambiguous information. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49(4), 244–51. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2011.01.004
White, S. S. W., Albano, A. M. A., Johnson, C. C. R., Kasari, C., Ollendick, T., Klin, A., Scahill, L. (2010). Development of a cognitive-behavioral intervention program to treat anxiety and social deficits in teens with high-functioning autism. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 13(1), 77–90. doi:10.1007/s10567-009-0062-3
Whittaker, A. A. E., & Robitschek, C. (2001). Multidimensional family functioning: Predicting personal growth initiative. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 48(4), 420–427. doi:10.1037//0022-0167.48.4.420
Wiborg, J. F., Knoop, H., Stulemeijer, M., Prins, J. B., & Bleijenberg, G. (2010). How does cognitive behaviour therapy reduce fatigue in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome? The role of physical activity. Psychological Medicine, 40(8), 1281–7. doi:10.1017/S0033291709992212
Wiborg, J. J. F., Knoop, H., Wensing, M., & Bleijenberg, G. (2012). Therapist effects and the dissemination of cognitive behavior therapy for chronic fatigue syndrome in community-based mental health care. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50(6), 393–6. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2012.03.002
Widiger, T. A., & Presnall, J. R. (2013). Clinical application of the five-factor model. Journal of Personality, 81(6), 515–27. doi:10.1111/jopy.12004
Wiggins, O. P., & Schwartz, M. A. (1991). Research Into Personality Disorders: The Alternatives of Dimensions and Ideal Types. Journal of Personality Disorders, 5(1), 69–81. doi:10.1521/pedi.1991.5.1.69
Wiggins, S., Elliott, R., & Cooper, M. (2012). The prevalence and characteristics of relational depth events in psychotherapy. Psychotherapy Research : Journal of the Society for Psychotherapy Research, 22(2), 139–58. doi:10.1080/10503307.2011.629635
Wilber, K. (2003). Waves, streams, states, and self: An outline of an integral psychology 1. The Humanistic Psychologist, 31(2-3), 22–49. doi:10.1080/08873267.2003.9986925
Wilber, K. (2004). Integral Psychology: Consciousness, Spirit, Psychology, Therapy. Nova Religio: The Journal of Alternative and Emergent Religions, 8(2), 125–127. doi:10.1525/nr.2004.8.2.125
Wilhelm, S., Phillips, K. A., Didie, E., Buhlmann, U., Greenberg, J. L., Fama, J. M., Steketee, G. (2014). Modular cognitive-behavioral therapy for body dysmorphic disorder: a randomized controlled trial. Behavior Therapy, 45(3), 314–27. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2013.12.007
Wilhelm, S., Phillips, K. A., Fama, J. J. M., Greenberg, J. L., & Steketee, G. (2011). Modular Cognitive–Behavioral Therapy for Body Dysmorphic Disorder. Behavior Therapy, 42(4), 624–33. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2011.02.002
Williams, A. A. M., Hundt, N. N. E., & Nelson-Gray, R. (2014). BIS and cognitive appraisals in predicting coping strategies. Personality and Individual Differences, 59, 60–64. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.11.006
Williams, J. M. G., Alatiq, Y., Crane, C., Barnhofer, T., Fennell, M. J. V, Duggan, D. S.,Goodwin, G. M. (2008). Mindfulness-based Cognitive Therapy (MBCT) in bipolar disorder: preliminary evaluation of immediate effects on between-episode functioning. Journal of Affective Disorders, 107(1-3), 275–9. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2007.08.022
Williams, R. M. R. (1994). Cognitive therapy for difficult patients-a review. International Review of Psychiatry, 6(2-3), 175–186. doi:10.3109/09540269409023257
Wilmoth, J. J. D., Adams-Price, C. E. C., Turner, J. J., Blaney, A. D., & Downey, L. (2014). Examining Social Connections as a Link Between Religious Participation and Well-being Among Older Adults. Journal of Religion, Spirituality & Aging, 26(2-3), 259–278. doi:10.1080/15528030.2013.867423
Wilt, J., Noftle, E. E. E., Fleeson, W., & Spain, J. J. S. (2012). The dynamic role of personality states in mediating the relationship between extraversion and positive affect. Journal of Personality, 80(5), 1205–36. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00756.x
Winkielman, P., & Schooler, J. W. (2011). Splitting consciousness: Unconscious, conscious, and metaconscious processes in social cognition. European Review of Social Psychology, 22(1), 1–35. doi:10.1080/10463283.2011.576580
Witter, R. A., Stock, W. A., Okun, M. A., & Haring, M. J. (1985). Religion and Subjective Well-Being in Adulthood: A Quantitative Synthesis. Review of Religious Research, 26(4), 332. doi:10.2307/3511048
Witthöft, M., & Hiller, W. (2010). Psychological Approaches to Origins and Treatments of Somatoform Disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6(1), 257–283. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131505
Wojcik, S. P., & Ditto, P. H. (2014). Motivated Happiness: Self-Enhancement Inflates Self-Reported Subjective Well-Being. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5(7), 825–834. doi:10.1177/1948550614534699
Wolbring, G. (2013). Subjective Wellbeing, Body-Related Ability Expectations and Peace. International Journal of Peace Studies, 18(2).
Wolbring, T., Keuschnigg, M., & Negele, E. (2013). Needs, Comparisons, and Adaptation: The Importance of Relative Income for Life Satisfaction. European Sociological Review, 29(1), 86–104. doi:10.1093/esr/jcr042
Wood, J. J. J., Fujii, C., Renno, P., Dyke, M. Van, & Van Dyke, M. (2014). Impact of Cognitive Behavioral Therapy on Observed Autism Symptom Severity During School Recess: A Preliminary Randomized, Controlled Trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(9), 2264–76. doi:10.1007/s10803-014-2097-7
Wood, J. V. J. (1989). Theory and research concerning social comparisons of personal attributes. Psychological Bulletin, 106(2), 231–248. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.106.2.231
Wrosch, C., Scheier, M. F., Miller, G. E., Schulz, R., & Carver, C. S. (2003). Adaptive self-regulation of unattainable goals: Goal disengagement, goal reengagement, and subjective well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(12), 1494-1508.
Wyrwich, K. K. W., Harnam, N., Locklear, J. C., Svedsäter, H., & Revicki, D. A. (2011). Understanding the relationships between health outcomes in generalized anxiety disorder clinical trials. Quality of Life Research : An International Journal of Quality of Life Aspects of Treatment, Care and Rehabilitation, 20(2), 255–62. doi:10.1007/s11136-010-9734-1
Yakeley, J., & Williams, A. (2014). Antisocial personality disorder: new directions. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, 20(2), 132–143. doi:10.1192/apt.bp.113.011205
Yakeley, J., & Wood, H. (2011). Patients’ experiences of forensic psychotherapy. Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy, 25(1), 105–114. doi:10.1080/02668734.2010.519160
Yook, K., Lee, S. H., Ryu, M., Kim, K. H. (2008). Usefulness of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for treating insomnia in patients with anxiety disorders: a pilot study. The Journal of nervous and mental disease, 196(6), 501-503.
Yoon, K. L., & Joormann, J. (2012). Is timing everything? Sequential effects of rumination and distraction on interpersonal problem solving. Cognitive therapy and research, 36(3), 165-172.
Yoon, S. L., Grundmann, O., Koepp, L., & Farrell, L. (2011). Management of irritable bowel syndrome (IBS) in adults: conventional and complementary/alternative approaches. Altern Med Rev, 16(2), 134-51.
Zalewski, M., Lengua, L. L. J., Wilson, A. A. C., Trancik, A., & Bazinet, A. (2011). Associations of coping and appraisal styles with emotion regulation during preadolescence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110(2), 141–58. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.03.001
Zhang, L., & Leung, J. J. P. (2002). Moderating effects of gender and age on the relationship between self-esteem and life satisfaction in mainland Chinese. International Journal of Psychology, 37(2), 83–91. doi:10.1080/00207560143000252
Zhang, R. R. P., & Tsingan, L. (2013). Extraversion and Neuroticism Mediate Associations Between Openness, Conscientiousness, and Agreeableness and Affective Well-Being. Journal of Happiness Studies. doi:10.1007/s10902-013-9482-3
Zhong, J. Y. J., & Mitchell, V. W. V. (2012). Does Consumer Well-Being Affect Hedonic Consumption? Psychology & Marketing, 29(8), 583–594. doi:10.1002/mar.20545
Zuzanek, J. (2013). Does Being Well-Off Make Us Happier? Problems of Measurement. Journal of Happiness Studies, 14(3), 795–815. doi:10.1007/s10902-012-9356-0
APPENDIX A – Positive Psychology Questionnaires
Subjective Happiness Scale (SHS)
Lyubomirsky, Ph.D.
Permission to use this questionnaire granted by Dr. Lyubomirsky by email on 10-25-2014.
WHAT THE QUESTIONNAIRE MEASURES:
The SHS is a 4-item scale of global subjective happiness. Two items ask respondents to characterize themselves using both absolute ratings and ratings relative to peers, whereas the other two items offer brief descriptions of happy and unhappy individuals and ask respondents the extent to which each characterization describes them. The SHS has been validated in 14 studies with a total of 2,732 participants. Data has been collected in the United States from students on two college campuses and one high school campus, from community adults in two California cities, and from older adults, as well as from students and community adults in Moscow, Russia. Results have indicated that the SHS has high internal consistency, which has been found to be stable across samples. Test-retest and self-peer correlations have suggested good to excellent reliability, and construct validation studies of convergent and discriminant validity have confirmed the use of this scale to measure the construct of subjective happiness.
Subjective Happiness Scale (SHS)
*1. Gender:
female male
*2. From the personality test your Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) was:
ISTJ ISFJ INFJ INTJ ISTP ISFP INFP INTP ESTP ESFP ENFP ENTP ESTJ ESFJ ENFJ ENTJ
*3. In general, I consider myself:
very unhappy unhappy slightly unhappy neither unhappy nor happy slightly happy happy very happy
*4. Compared to most of my peers, I consider myself:
very unhappy unhappy slightly unhappy neither unhappy nor happy slightly happy happy very happy
*5. Some people are generally very happy. They enjoy life regardless of what is going on, getting the
most out of everything. To what extent does this characterization describe you?
Absolutely Untrue Mostly Untrue Somewhat Untrue Can’t Say True or False Somewhat True Mostly True Absolutely True
*6. Some people are generally not very happy. Although they are not depressed, they never seem as happy as they might be. To what extent does this characterization describe you?
Absolutely Untrue Mostly Untrue Somewhat Untrue Can’t Say True or False Somewhat True Mostly True Absolutely True
REFERENCES:
(Lyubomirsky & Lepper, 1999; Lyubomirsky & Ross, 1997, 1999; Lyubomirsky, 2001)
Personal Growth Initiative Scale (PGIS)
Christine Robitschek, Ph.D.
Permission to use this questionnaire granted by Dr. Robitschek by email on 10-25-2014.
WHAT THE QUESTIONNAIRE MEASURES:
The PGIS is a self-report instrument that yields a single scale score for personal growth initiative. Personal growth initiative is a person’s active and intentional involvement in changing and developing as a person. The PGIS consists of nine items that are rated on a Likert scale from 1 = Strongly Disagree to 6 = Strongly Agree. Item scores are summed to obtain a total PGI score. There is evidence that the PGIS is strongly positively related to psychological well-being and negatively related to psychological distress. Reliability and validity evidence has been strong. The PGIS takes about 5 minutes to complete, and there is no time limit.
Personal Growth Initiative Scale
*1. From the personality test your Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) was:
ISTJ ISFJ INFJ INTJ ISTP ISFP INFP INTP ESTP ESFP ENFP ENTP ESTJ ESFJ ENFJ ENTJ
*2. I know how to change specific things that I want to change in my life.
Definitely disagree Mostly disagree Somewhat disagree Somewhat agree Mostly agree Definitely agree
*3. I have a good sense of where I am headed in my life.
Definitely disagree Mostly disagree Somewhat disagree Somewhat agree Mostly agree Definitely agree
*4. If I want to change something in my life, I initiate the transition process.
Definitely disagree Mostly disagree Somewhat disagree Somewhat agree Mostly agree Definitely agree
*5. I can choose the role that I want to have in a group.
Definitely disagree Mostly disagree Somewhat disagree Somewhat agree Mostly agree Definitely agree
*6. I know what I need to do to get started toward reaching my goals.
Definitely disagree Mostly disagree Somewhat disagree Somewhat agree Mostly agree Definitely agree
*7. I have a specific action plan to help me reach my goals.
Definitely disagree Mostly disagree Somewhat disagree Somewhat agree Mostly agree Definitely agree
*8. I take charge of my life.
Definitely disagree Mostly disagree Somewhat disagree Somewhat agree Mostly agree Definitely agree
*9. I know what my unique contribution to the world might be.
Definitely disagree Mostly disagree Somewhat disagree Somewhat agree Mostly agree Definitely agree
*10. I have a plan for making my life more balanced.
Definitely disagree Mostly disagree Somewhat disagree Somewhat agree Mostly agree Definitely agree
References:
(Robitschek & Ashton, 2012; Robitschek, 1998; Robitschek & Cook, 1999; Robitschek & Kashubeck, 1999; Whittaker & Robitschek, 2001)
Meaning in Life Questionnaire (MLQ)
Michael F. Steger, Patricia Frazier, Shigehiro Oishi, and Matthew Kaler
Permission to use this questionnaire granted by Dr. Steger by email on 10-25-2014.
WHAT THE QUESTIONNAIRE MEASURES:
The Meaning in Life Questionnaire assesses two dimensions of meaning in life using 10 items rated on a seven-point scale from “Absolutely True” to “Absolutely Untrue.” The Presence of Meaning subscale measures how full respondents feel their lives are of meaning. The Search for Meaning subscale measures how engaged and motivated respondents are in efforts to find meaning or deepen their understanding of meaning in their lives. The MLQ has good reliability, test-retest stability, stable factor structure, and convergence among informants. Presence is positively related to well-being, intrinsic religiosity, extraversion and agreeableness, and negatively related to anxiety and depression. Search is positively related to religious quest, rumination, past-negative and present-fatalistic time perspectives, negative affect, depression, and neuroticism, and negatively related to future time perspective, close-mindedness (dogmatism), and well-being. Presence relates as expected with personal growth self-appraisals, and altruistic and spiritual behaviors as assessed through daily diaries. The MLQ takes about 5 minutes to complete.
MLQ
Please take a moment to think about what makes your life and existence feel important and significant to you. Please respond to the following statements as truthfully and accurately as you can, and also please remember that these are very subjective questions and that there are no right or wrong answers.
Meaning in Life (Search) Questionnaire
*1. Gender:
female male
*2. From the personality test your Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) was:
ISTJ ISFJ INFJ INTJ ISTP ISFP INFP INTP ESTP ESFP ENFP ENTP ESTJ ESFJ ENFJ ENTJ
*3. I am looking for something that makes my life feel meaningful
Absolutely Untrue Mostly Untrue Somewhat Untrue Can’t Say True or False Somewhat True Mostly True Absolutely True
*4. I am always looking to find my life’s purpose.
Absolutely Untrue Mostly Untrue Somewhat Untrue Can’t Say True or False Somewhat True Mostly True Absolutely True
*5. I am always searching for something that makes my life feel significant.
Absolutely Untrue Mostly Untrue Somewhat Untrue Can’t Say True or False Somewhat True Mostly True Absolutely True
*6. I am seeking a purpose or mission for my life.
Absolutely Untrue Mostly Untrue Somewhat Untrue Can’t Say True or False Somewhat True Mostly True Absolutely True
*7. I am searching for meaning in my life.
Absolutely Untrue Mostly Untrue Somewhat Untrue Can’t Say True or False Somewhat True Mostly True Absolutely True
Meaning in Life (Presence) Questionnaire
*1. Gender:
female male
*2. From the personality test your Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) was:
ISTJ ISFJ INFJ INTJ ISTP ISFP INFP INTP ESTP ESFP ENFP ENTP ESTJ ESFJ ENFJ ENTJ
*3. I understand my life’s meaning.
Absolutely Untrue Mostly Untrue Somewhat Untrue Can’t Say True or False Somewhat True Mostly True Absolutely True
*4. My life has a clear sense of purpose.
Absolutely Untrue Mostly Untrue Somewhat Untrue Can’t Say True or False Somewhat True Mostly True Absolutely True
*5. I have discovered a satisfying life purpose.
Absolutely Untrue Mostly Untrue Somewhat Untrue Can’t Say True or False Somewhat True Mostly True Absolutely True
*6. I have a good sense of what makes my life meaningful.
Absolutely Untrue Mostly Untrue Somewhat Untrue Can’t Say True or False Somewhat True Mostly True Absolutely True
*7. My life has no clear purpose.
Absolutely Untrue Mostly Untrue Somewhat Untrue Can’t Say True or False Somewhat True Mostly True Absolutely True
Note – #7 is reverse-coded
REFERENCES:
(Steger et al., 2006; Steger & Frazier, 2005)
Satisfaction with Life Scale
Ed Diener, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign
Permission to use this questionnaire granted by Dr. Diener by email on 10-25-2014.
WHAT THE QUESTIONNAIRE MEASURES:
The Satisfaction with Life Scale was developed to assess satisfaction with people’s lives as a whole. The scale does not assess satisfaction with specific life domains, such as health or finances, but allows subjects to integrate and weigh these domains in whatever way they choose. It takes only a few minutes to complete.
Satisfaction with Life Scale
*1. Gender:
female male
*2. From the personality test your Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) was:
ISTJ ISFJ INFJ INTJ ISTP ISFP INFP INTP ESTP ESFP ENFP ENTP ESTJ ESFJ ENFJ ENTJ
*3. In most ways my life is close to my ideal.
strongly disagree disagree slightly disagree neutral slightly agree agree strongly agree
*4. The conditions of my life are excellent.
strongly disagree disagree slightly disagree neutral slightly agree agree strongly agree
*5. I am satisfied with life.
strongly disagree disagree slightly disagree neutral slightly agree agree strongly agree
*6. So far I have gotten the important things I want in life.
strongly disagree disagree slightly disagree neutral slightly agree agree strongly agree
*7. If I could live my life over, I would change almost nothing.
strongly disagree disagree slightly disagree neutral slightly agree agree strongly agree
REFERENCES:
(Diener, Emmons, Larson et al., 1985; Pavot et al., 1991; Pavot & Diener, 1993)
Gratitude Questionnaire
Michael E. McCullough, Ph.D., Robert A. Emmons, Ph.D., Jo-Ann Tsang, Ph.D.
Permission to use this questionnaire granted by Dr. McCullough by email on 10-25-2014.
WHAT THE QUESTIONNAIRE MEASURES:
The GQ-6 is a short, self-report measure of the disposition to experience gratitude. Participants answer 6 items on a 1 to 7 scale (1 = “strongly disagree”, 7 = “strongly agree”). Two items are reverse-scored to inhibit response bias. The GQ-6 has good internal reliability, with alphas between .82 and .87, and there is evidence that the GQ-6 is positively related to optimism, life satisfaction, hope, spirituality and religiousness, forgiveness, empathy and prosocial behavior, and negatively related to depression, anxiety, materialism and envy. The GQ-6 takes less than 5 minutes to complete, but there is no time limit.
Gratitude Questionnaire
*1. Gender:
female male
*2. From the personality test your Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) was:
ISTJ ISFJ INFJ INTJ ISTP ISFP INFP INTP ESTP ESFP ENFP ENTP ESTJ ESFJ ENFJ ENTJ
*3. I have so much in life to be thankful for.
strongly disagree disagree slightly disagree neutral slightly agree agree strongly agree
*4. If I had to list everything that I felt grateful for, it would be a very long list.
strongly disagree disagree slightly disagree neutral slightly agree agree strongly agree
*5. When I look at the world, I don’t see much to be grateful for.
strongly disagree disagree slightly disagree neutral slightly agree agree strongly agree
*6. I am grateful to a wide variety of people.
strongly disagree disagree slightly disagree neutral slightly agree agree strongly agree
*7. As I get older I find myself more able to appreciate the people, events, and situations
that have been part of my life history.
strongly disagree disagree slightly disagree neutral slightly agree agree strongly agree
*8. Long amounts of time can go by before I feel grateful to something or someone.
strongly disagree disagree slightly disagree neutral slightly agree agree strongly agree
REFERENCES:
(Emmons & Mishra, 2011; McCullough et al., 2002)
APPENDIX B – Informed Consent For Participants
California Institute for Human Science
Department of Psychology
Study Name: A PILOT STUDY EXPLORING THE MYERS-BRIGGS TYPE INDICATOR AS A PREDICTOR OF SUCCESS IN COGNITIVE BEHAVIORAL THERAPY
Principal Investigator: Dan Hurley
PLEASE READ THIS DOCUMENT CAREFULLY.
YOU MUST BE AT LEAST 18 YEARS OF AGE TO GIVE YOUR CONSENT TO PARTICIPATE IN RESEARCH.
Research participation is voluntary, and you have the right to withdraw at any time, without prejudice, should you object to the nature of the research. You are entitled to ask questions and to receive an explanation after your participation.
Description of the Study
This is a study in which a number of different kinds of psychological tests and measures are being evaluated. To do this, I will ask you to do the following:
1. A Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) test will be administered via spreadsheet at the beginning of the study.
2. The following questionnaires will be filled out before exposure to Rational Emotive Behavior/Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (REBT/CBT) concepts:
Meaning in Life (Search) Questionnaire (Before) at
http://www.surveymonkey.com/s/X8KG6JG
Meaning in Life (Presence) Questionnaire (Before) http://www.surveymonkey.com/s/XK55TDB
Gratitude Questionnaire (Before) at
http://www.surveymonkey.com/s/XKZFGQN
Satisfaction with Life Scale (Before) at
http://www.surveymonkey.com/s/KFH833H
Personal Growth Initiative Scale (Before) at
http://www.surveymonkey.com/s/CSHQFPB
Subjective Happiness Scale (Before) at
http://www.surveymonkey.com/s/X88QB2C
3. Rational Emotive Behavior/Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (REBT/CBT) concepts are found at:
http://dhurley.com/CBT/CBT1.docx
http://dhurley.com/CBT/CBT2.docx
http://dhurley.com/CBT/CBT3.docx
http://dhurley.com/CBT/CBT4.docx
http://dhurley.com/CBT/CBT5.docx
http://dhurley.com/CBT/CBT6.docx
http://dhurley.com/CBT/CBT7.docx
http://dhurley.com/CBT/CBT8.docx
http://dhurley.com/CBT/CBT9.docx
http://dhurley.com/CBT/CBT10.docx
http://dhurley.com/CBT/CBT11.docx
http://dhurley.com/CBT/CBT12.docx
4. The following questionnaires will be filled out after exposure to Rational Emotive Behavior/Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (REBT/CBT) concepts:
Gratitude Questionnaire (After) at
http://www.surveymonkey.com/s/BMDQLZP;
Satisfaction with Life Scale (After) at
http://www.surveymonkey.com/s/BMD38QN;
Personal Growth Initiative Scale (After) at
http://www.surveymonkey.com/s/BMYK5PW;
Subjective Happiness Scale (After) at
http://www.surveymonkey.com/s/BMYRQTX;
Meaning in Life (Search) Questionnaire (After) at
http://www.surveymonkey.com/s/BM272S9
Meaning in Life (Presence) Questionnaire (After) at http://www.surveymonkey.com/s/BM2B9JG
Purpose of the Study
This study was designed to test the hypothesis that a relationship exists between Jungian psychological types, as measured by the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator of Personality Variables, and the utility of Cognitive Behavioral therapy restructuring concepts (Appendix D) to produce psychological growth. This will be measured by the following assessments: Subjective Happiness Scale (SHS), Personal Growth Initiative Scale (PGIS), Meaning in Life Questionnaire (MLQ), Satisfaction with Life Scale, and Gratitude Questionnaire (Appendix A).
Possible Risks
a) When filling out questionnaires, you may come across a question or answer choice that you find unpleasant, upsetting, or otherwise objectionable. For instance, a few of the questions may cause you to think about negative emotional states.
b) You may feel that you have performed poorly on a test. For the questionnaires I am evaluating, there are no right or wrong answers.
Possible Benefits
a) When your participation is complete, you will be given an opportunity to learn about this research, which may be useful to you in your course or in understanding yourself and others.
b) You will have an opportunity to contribute to psychological science by participating in this research.
Compensation for your Time:
You will receive extra credit in your computer science course. You will receive extra credit simply by virtue of taking the online questionnaires. You are free to withdraw your participation at any time without penalty. Alternative means of earning extra credit are available. Please consult your class syllabus for information regarding alternative means of earning credit or extra credit in your course.
Confidentiality
Your identity will be protected. All data will be kept in secured files, in accord with Federal regulations, and the American Psychological Association. All identifying information will be removed from questionnaires as soon as your participation is complete. Additionally, all emails and survey questionnaires will be deleted. No one will be able to know which are your questionnaire responses.
Finally, remember that it is no individual person’s responses that interests me; I am studying the usefulness of the tests in question for people in general.
Opportunities to Ask Questions
Any technical questions about this research may be directed to:
Principal Investigator: Dan Hurley Email: DanHurley@dhurley.com
Opportunities to Withdraw at Will
If you decide now or at any point to withdraw this consent or stop participating, you are free to do so at no penalty to yourself. You are free to skip specific questions and continue participating at no penalty.
Additionally, if you find that you are experiencing any negative effects from participation in this study, you may contact Santa Monica Colleges on campus psychological services in the Liberal Arts Building, Room 110, (310) 434-4503.
Opportunities to be Informed of Results
In all likelihood, the results will be fully available around 9-1-2015. Preliminary results will be available earlier. If you wish to be told the results of this research, please contact:
Principal Investigator: Dan Hurley Email: DanHurley@dhurley.com
I will either meet with you or direct you to where you can read a copy of the results. In addition, there is a chance that the results from this study will be published in a scientific psychology journal, which would be available in many libraries. In such an article, participants would be identified in general terms as undergraduate students at a large community college.
Your response to the online questionnaires mentioned above indicates that you voluntarily agree to participate in this study.
APPENDIX C – Myers-Briggs Assessment
The following abbreviated Myers-Briggs Excel spreadsheet was developed by Dr. Richard Scholl (Leonard, Beauvais, & Scholl, 1995; Leonard, Scholl, & Kowalski, 1999). Permission to use the spreadsheet granted by Dr. Scholl by email on 10-24-2014.
http://dhurley.com/MBTIHurley.xls
APPENDIX D – Presentation on CBT/REBT
(Branch & Willson, 2010).
Note: Study participants will be sent a copy of the following presentation of CBT concepts. They will be asked to read and summarize in their own words what they mean and how they might apply them in their own lives.
CBT 1
A (actuator activating event) + B (beliefs and meanings about the event) =
C (emotional and behavioral consequence)
✓ A is the activating event. An activating event means a real external event that has occurred, a future event that you anticipate occurring or an internal event in your mind, such as an image, memory or dream.
The ‘A’ is often referred to as your ‘trigger’.
✓ B is your beliefs. Your beliefs include your thoughts, your personal rules, the demands you make (on yourself, the world and other people) and the meanings that you attach to external and internal events.
✓ C is the consequences. Consequences include your emotions, behaviors and physical sensations that accompany different emotions.
Catastrophising: Turning Mountains Back Into Molehills
Catastrophising is taking a relatively minor negative event and imagining all sorts of disasters resulting from that one small event.
Consider these examples of catastrophising:
✓ You’re at a party and you accidentally stumble headlong into a flower arrangement. After you extract yourself from the foliage, you scurry home and conclude that everyone at the party witnessed your little trip and laughed at you.
✓ You’re waiting for your teenage daughter to return home after an evening at the cinema with friends. The clock strikes 10:00 p.m., and you hear no reassuring rattle of her key in the door. By 10:05 p.m., you start imagining her accepting a lift home from a friend who drives recklessly.
At 10:10 p.m., you’re convinced she’s been involved in a head-on collision and paramedics are at the scene. By 10:15 p.m., you’re weeping over her grave.
Spotting Errors in Your Thinking
✓ Your new partner declines an invitation to have dinner with your parents. Before giving him a chance to explain his reasons, you put down the phone and decide that this is his way of telling you the relationship’s over. Furthermore, you imagine that right now he’s ringing friends and telling them what a mistake it was dating you. You decide you’re never going to find another partner and will die old and lonely.
Catastrophising leads many an unfortunate soul to misinterpret a social faux pas as a social disaster, a late arrival as a car accident or a minor disagreement as total rejection.
Nip catastrophic thinking in the bud by recognizing it for what it is – just thoughts. When you find yourself thinking of the worst possible scenario, try the following strategies:
✓ Put your thoughts in perspective. Even if everyone at the party did see your flower-arranging act, are you sure no one was sympathetic?
Surely you aren’t the only person in the world to have tripped over in public. Chances are, people are far less interested in your embarrassing moment than you think. Falling over at a party isn’t great, but in the grand scheme of things it’s hardly society-page news.
✓ Consider less terrifying explanations. What other reasons are there for your daughter being late? Isn’t being late for curfew a common feature of adolescence? Perhaps the film ran over, or she got caught up chat-ting and forgot the time. Don’t get so absorbed in extreme emotions that you’re startled to find your daughter in the doorway apologizing about missing the bus.
✓ Weigh up the evidence. Do you have enough information to conclude that your partner wants to leave you? Has he given you any reason to think this before? Look for evidence that contradicts your catastrophic assumption. For example, have you had more enjoyable times together than not?
✓ Focus on what you can do to cope with the situation, and the people or resources that can come to your aid. Engaging in a few more social encounters can help you put your party faux pas behind you. You can repair a damaged relationship – or find another. Even an injury following an accident can be fixed with medical care.
No matter how great a travesty you create in your mind, the world’s unlikely to end because of it – even if the travesty comes to pass. You’re probably far more capable of surviving embarrassing and painful events than you give yourself credit for – human beings can be very resilient.
CBT 2
All-or-Nothing Thinking: Finding Somewhere In-between
All-or-nothing or black-or-white thinking is extreme thinking that can lead to extreme emotions and behaviors. People either love you or hate you, right? Something’s either perfect or a disaster. You’re either responsibility-free or totally to blame? Sound sensible? We hope not!
All-or-nothing thinking.
Unfortunately, humans fall into the all-or-nothing trap all too easily:
✓ Imagine you’re trying to eat healthily in order to lose weight and you cave in to the temptation of a doughnut. All-or-nothing thinking may lead you to conclude that your plan is in ruins and then to go on to eat the other 11 doughnuts in the pack.
✓ You’re studying a degree course and you fail one module. All-or-nothing thinking makes you decide that the whole endeavor is pointless. Either you get the course totally right or it’s just a write-off.
Consider the humble thermometer as your guide to overcoming the tendency of all-or-nothing thinking. A thermometer reads degrees of temperature, not only ‘hot’ and ‘cold’. Think like a thermometer – in degrees, not extremes. You can use the following pointers to help you change your thinking:
Spotting Errors in Your Thinking
✓ Be realistic. You can’t possibly get through life without making mistakes. One doughnut doesn’t a diet ruin. Remind yourself of your goal, forgive yourself for the minor slip, and resume your diet.
✓ Develop ‘both–and’ reasoning skills. An alternative to all-or-nothing thinking is both–and reasoning. You need to mentally allow two seeming opposites to exist together. You can both succeed in your overall educational goals and fail a test or two. Life is not a case of being either a success or a failure. You can both assume that you’re an okay person as you are and strive to change.
All-or-nothing thinking can sabotage goal-directed behavior. You’re far more likely to throw in the towel at the first sign of something blocking your goal when you refuse to allow a margin for error. Beware of ‘either/or’ statements and global labels such as ‘good’ and ‘bad’ or ‘success’ and ‘failure’. Neither people nor life situations are often that cut and dry.
CBT 3
Fortune-Telling: Stepping Away From the Crystal Ball
Introducing CBT Basics
✓ You’ve been feeling a bit depressed lately and you aren’t enjoying your-self like you used to. Someone from work invites you to a party, but you decide that if you go you won’t have a good time. The food will unpalatable, the music will be irksome and the other guests are sure to find you boring. So, you opt to stay in and bemoan the state of your social life.
✓ You fancy the bloke who sells you coffee every morning on the way to the office, and you’d like to go out with him on a date. You predict that if you ask him, you’ll be so anxious that you’ll say something stupid.
Anyway, he’s bound to say no thanks – someone that attractive must surely be in a relationship.
✓ You always thought that hang-gliding would be fun, but you’ve got an anxious disposition. If you try the sport, you’re sure to lose your nerve at the last minute and just end up wasting your time and money.
You’re better off letting the future unfold without trying to guess how it may turn out. Put the dustcover back on the crystal ball, flog the Ouija board on eBay, leave the tarot cards alone and try the following strategies instead:
✓ Test out your predictions. You really never know how much fun you might have at a party until you get there – and the food could be amazing. Maybe the chap at the coffee shop has got a partner, but you won’t be sure until you ask.
✓ Be prepared to take risks. Isn’t it worth possibly losing a bit of cash for the opportunity to try a sport you’ve always been interested in?
And can’t you bear the possibility of appearing a trifle nervous for the chance to get to know someone you really like? There’s a saying ‘a ship is safe in a harbor’, but that’s not what ships are built for. Learning to live experimentally and taking calculated risks is a recipe for keeping life interesting and rewarding.
✓ Understand that your past experiences don’t determine your future experiences. Just because the last party you went to turned out to be a dreary homage to the seventies, the last person you asked out went a bit green, and that scuba-diving venture resulted in a severe case of the bends doesn’t mean that you’ll never have better luck again.
Typically, fortune-telling stops you from taking action. It can also become a bit of a self-fulfilling prophecy. If you keep telling yourself that you won’t enjoy that party, you’re liable to make that prediction come true. Same goes for meeting new people and trying new things. So, put on your party gear, ask him out for dinner and book yourself in for some hang-gliding.
CBT 4
Mind-Reading: Taking Your Guesses with a Pinch of Salt
So, you think you know what other people are thinking, do you? With mind-reading, the tendency is often to assume that others are thinking negative things about you or have negative motives and intentions.
Here are some examples of mind-reading tendencies:
✓ You’re chatting with someone and they look over your shoulder as you’re speaking, break eye contact and (perish the thought) yawn. You conclude immediately that the other person thinks your conversation is mind-numbing and that he’d rather be talking to someone else.
✓ Your boss advises that you book some time off to use up your annual leave. You decide that he’s saying this because he thinks your work is rubbish and wants the opportunity to interview for your replacement while you’re on leave.
✓ You pass a neighbor on the street. He says a quick hello but doesn’t look very friendly or pleased to see you. You think that he must be annoyed with you about your dog howling at the last full moon and is making plans to report you to environmental health.
You can never know for certain what another person is thinking, so you’re wise to pour salt on your negative assumptions. Stand back and take a look at all the evidence to hand. Take control of your tendency to mind-read by trying the following:
✓ Generate some alternative reasons for what you see. The person you’re chatting with may be tired, be preoccupied with his own thoughts or just have spotted someone he knows.
✓ Consider that your guesses may be wrong. Are your fears really about your boss’s motives, or do they concern your own insecurity about your abilities at work? Do you have enough information or hard evidence to conclude that your boss thinks your work is substandard? Does it follow logically that ‘consider booking time off’ means ‘you’re getting the sack’?
✓ Get more information (if appropriate).Ask your neighbor whether your dog kept him up all night, and think of some ways to muffle your pet next time the moon waxes.
You tend to mind-read what you fear most. Mind-reading is a bit like putting a slide in a slide projector. What you project or imagine is going on in other people’s minds is very much based on what’s already in yours.
CBT 5
Emotional Reasoning: Reminding Yourself That Feelings Aren’t Facts
Surely we’re wrong about this one. Surely your feelings are real hard evidence of the way things are? Actually, no! Often, relying too heavily on your feelings as a guide leads you off the reality path. Here are some examples of emotional reasoning:
✓ Your partner has been spending long nights at the office with a co-worker for the past month. You feel jealous and suspicious of your partner. Based on these feelings, you conclude that your partner’s having an affair with his co-worker.
✓ You feel guilty out of the blue. You conclude that you must have done something wrong otherwise you wouldn’t be feeling guilty.
✓ You wake up feeling anxious, with a vague sense of dread. You assume that there must be something seriously wrong in your life and search your mind frantically for the source of your ill- feeling.
Often your feelings are simply due to a thought or memory that you may not even be totally aware of having had. Other times they can be symptoms of another disorder such as depression or anxiety problems.
Some of the feelings you experience on waking are left over from dreams that you may or may not remember. As a rule of thumb, it pays to be somewhat skeptical about the validity of your feelings in the first instance. Your feelings can be misleading.
When you spot emotional reasoning taking over your thoughts, take a step back and try the following:
1. Take notice of your thoughts. Take notice of thoughts such as ‘I’m feeling nervous, something must be wrong’ and ‘I’m so angry, and that really shows how badly you’ve behaved’, and recognize that feelings are not always the best measure of reality, especially if you’re not in the best emotional shape at the moment.
2. Ask yourself how you’d view the situation if you were feeling calmer.
Look to see if there is any concrete evidence to support your interpretation of your feelings. For example, is there really any evidence that something bad is going to happen?
3. Give yourself time to allow your feelings to subside. When you’re feeling calmer, review your conclusions and remember that it is quite possible that your feelings are the consequence of your present emotional state (or even just fatigue) rather than indicators of the state of reality.
4. If you can’t find any obvious and immediate source of your unpleasant feelings – overlook them. Get into the shower despite your sense of dread, for example. If a concrete reason to be anxious does exist, it won’t get dissolved in the shower. If your anxiety is all smoke and mirrors, you may well find it washes down the drain.
The problem with viewing your feelings as factual is that you stop looking for contradictory information – or for any additional information at all. Balance your emotional reasoning with a little more looking at the facts that support and contradict your views.
CBT 6
Overgeneralizing: Avoiding the Part/Whole Error
Overgeneralizing is the error of drawing global conclusions from one or more events. When you find yourself thinking ‘always’, ‘never’, ‘people are . . .’ or ‘the world’s . . .’, you may well be overgeneralizing.
You might recognize overgeneralizing in the following examples:
✓ You feel down. When you get into your car to go to work, it doesn’t start.
You think to yourself, ‘Things like this are always happening to me.
Nothing ever goes right’, which makes you feel even more gloomy.
✓ You become angry easily. Travelling to see a friend, you’re delayed by a fellow passenger who cannot find the money to pay her train fare. You think, ‘This is typical! Other people are just so stupid’, and you become tense and angry.
✓ You tend to feel guilty easily. You yell at your child for not understanding his homework and then decide that you’re a thoroughly rotten parent.
Situations are rarely so stark or extreme that they merit terms like ‘always’ and ‘never’. Rather than overgeneralizing, consider the following:
Spotting Errors in Your Thinking
✓ Get a little perspective. How true is the thought that nothing ever goes right for you? How many other people in the world may be having car trouble at this precise moment?
✓ Suspend judgement. When you judge all people as stupid, including the poor creature waiting in line for the train, you make yourself more out-raged and are less able to deal effectively with a relatively minor hiccup.
✓ Be specific. Would you be a totally rotten parent for losing patience with your child? Can you legitimately conclude that one incident of poor parenting cancels out all the good things you do for your little one? Perhaps your impatience is simply an area you need to target for improvement.
Shouting at your child in a moment of stress no more makes you a rotten parent than singing him a favorite lullaby makes you a perfect parent.
Condemning yourself on the basis of making a mistake does nothing to solve the problem, so be specific and steer clear of global conclusions. Change what you think you can and need to but also forgive yourself (and others) for singular errors or misdeeds.
CBT 7
Labeling: Giving Up the Rating Game
Labels, and the process of labeling people and events, are everywhere. For example, people who have low self-esteem may label themselves as ‘worthless’, ‘inferior’ or ‘inadequate’
If you label other people as ‘no good’ or ‘useless’, you’re likely to become angry with them. Or perhaps you label the world as ‘unsafe’ or ‘totally unfair’? The error here is that you’re globally rating things that are too complex for a definitive label. The following are examples of labeling:
✓ You read a distressing article in the newspaper about a rise in crime in your city. The article activates your belief that you live in a thoroughly dangerous place, which contributes to you feeling anxious about going out.
✓ You receive a poor mark for an essay. You start to feel low and label yourself as a failure.
✓ You become angry when someone cuts in front of you in a traffic queue.
You label the other driver as a total loser for his bad driving.
Strive to avoid labeling yourself, other people and the world around you.
Accept that they’re complex and ever. Recognize evidence that doesn’t fit your labels, in order to help you weaken your conviction in your global rating. For example:
✓ Allow for varying degrees. Think about it: The world isn’t a dangerous place but rather a place that has many different aspects with varying degrees of safety and risk.
✓ Celebrate complexities. All human beings – yourself included – are unique, multifaceted and ever-changing. To label yourself as a failure on the strength of one failing is an extreme form of overgeneralizing.
Likewise, other people are just as complex and unique as you. One bad action doesn’t equal a bad person.
When you label a person or aspect of the world in a global way, you exclude potential for change and improvement. Accepting yourself as you are is a powerful first step towards self-improvement.
CBT 8
Making Demands: Thinking Flexibly
Albert Ellis, founder of rational emotive behavior therapy, one of the first cognitive-behavioral therapies, places demands at the very heart of emotional problems. Thoughts and beliefs that contain words like ‘must’, ‘should’, ‘need’, ‘ought’, ‘got to’ and ‘have to’ are often problematic because they’re extreme and rigid.
The inflexibility of the demands you place on yourself, the world around you and other people often means you don’t adapt to reality as well as you could.
Consider these possible examples:
✓ You believe that you must have the approval of your friends and colleagues. This leads you to feel anxious in many social situations and drives you to try to win everyone’s approval – possibly at great personal cost.
✓ You think that because you try very hard to be kind and considerate to others, they really ought to be just as kind and considerate in return.
Because your demand is not realistic – sadly, other people are governed by their own priorities – you often feel hurt about your friends (or even strangers) not acting the way you do yourself.
✓ You believe that you absolutely should never let people down. Therefore, you rarely put your own welfare first. At work, you do more than your fair share because you don’t assert yourself, and so you often end up feeling stressed and depressed.
Holding flexible preferences about yourself, other people and the world in general is the healthy alternative to inflexible rules and demands. Rather than making demands on yourself, the world and others, try the following techniques:
✓ Pay attention to language. Replace words like ‘must’, ‘need’ and ‘should’ with ‘prefer’, ‘wish’ and ‘want’.
✓ Limit approval seeking. Can you manage to have a satisfying life even if you don’t get the approval of everyone you seek it from? Specifically, you’ll feel more confident in social situations if you recognize your preference for approval rather than viewing approval as a dire need.
✓ Understand that the world doesn’t play to your rules. In fact, other people tend to have their own rulebooks. So, no matter how much you value considerate behavior, your friends may not give it the same value. If you can give others the right to not live up to your standards, you’ll feel less hurt when they fail to do so.
✓ Retain your standards, ideals and preferences, and ditch your rigid demands about how you, others and the world ‘have to’ be. So keep acting consistently with how you would like things to be rather than becoming depressed or irate about things not being the way you believe they must be.
When you hold rigid demands about the way things ‘have got to be’, you have no margin for deviation or error. You leave yourself vulnerable to experiencing exaggerated emotional disturbance when things in life just don’t go your way.
CBT 9
Mental Filtering: Keeping an Open Mind
Mental filtering is a bias in the way you process information, in which you acknowledge only information that fits with a belief you hold. The process is much like a filter on a camera lens that allows in only certain kinds of light.
Information that doesn’t fit tends to be ignored. If you think any of the following, you’re making the ‘mental filtering’ thinking error:
✓ You believe you’re a failure, so you tend to focus on your mistakes at work and overlook successes and achievements. At the end of the week, you often feel disappointed about your lack of achievement – but this is probably largely the result of you not paying attention to your successes.
✓ You believe you’re unlikeable, and really notice each time your friend is late to call back or seems too busy to see you. You tend to disregard the ways in which people act warmly towards you, thus sustaining your view that you’re unlikeable.
To combat mental filtering, look more closely at situations you feel down about. Deliberately collecting evidence that contradicts your negative thoughts can help you to correct your information-processing bias. Try the following:
✓ Examine your filters closely. For example, are you sifting your achievements through an ‘I’m a failure’ filter? If so, then only failure-related information gets through. If you look for a friend’s achievements over the same week without a filter, you’re likely to find him in far greater possession of success. So drop the filter when assessing yourself in the same way you do when looking at your friends’ achievements.
Spotting Errors in Your Thinking
✓ Gather evidence. Imagine you’re collecting evidence for a court case to prove that your negative thought isn’t true. What evidence do you cite? Would, for example, an assertion that you’re unlikeable stand up in court against the proof of your friends behaving warmly towards you?
If you only ever take in information that fits with your negative thinking, you can very easily end up reinforcing undesirable thinking habits. The fact that you don’t see the positive stuff about yourself, or your experiences, doesn’t mean it isn’t there.
CBT 10
Disqualifying the Positive: Keeping the Baby When Throwing Out the Bathwater
Disqualifying the positive is related to the biased way that people can process information. Disqualifying the positive is a mental response to a positive event that transforms it into a neutral or negative event.
The following are examples of disqualifying the positive:
✓ You convince yourself that you’re worthless and unlovable. You respond to a work promotion by thinking, ‘This doesn’t count, because anyone could get this sort of thing.’ The result: Instead of feeling pleased, you feel quite disappointed.
✓ You think you’re pathetic and feel low. A friend tells you you’re a very good friend, but you disqualify this in your mind by thinking, ‘She’s only saying that because she feels sorry for me. I really am pathetic.’
Hone your skills for accepting compliments and acknowledging your good points. You can try the following strategies to improve your skills:
✓ Become aware of your responses to positive ‘data’. Practise acknowledging and accepting positive feedback and acknowledging good points about yourself, others and the world. For example, you could override your workplace disappointment by recognizing that you’re the one who got the promotion. You can even consider that the promotion may well have been a result of your hard work.
✓ Practice accepting a compliment graciously with a simple thank you.
Rejecting a sincerely delivered compliment is rather like turning down a gift. Steer your thinking towards taking in positive experiences. When others point out attributes you have, start deliberately making a note of those good points. Even if your current thinking bias leads you to doubt the validity of a compliment or good experience, try considering that you may well be wrong to do so. Trust what others say for a change!
If you frequently disqualify or distort your positive attributes or experiences, you can easily sustain a negative belief, even in the face of overwhelming positive evidence.
CBT 11
Low Frustration Tolerance: Realizing You Can Bear the ‘Unbearable’
Low frustration tolerance refers to the error of assuming that when something’s difficult to tolerate, it’s ‘intolerable’. This thinking error means magnifying discomfort and not tolerating temporary discomfort when it’s in your interest to do so for longer-term benefit.
The following are examples of low frustration tolerance:
✓ You often procrastinate on college assignments, thinking, ‘It’s just too much hassle. I’ll do it later when I feel more in the mood.’ You tend to wait until the assignment’s nearly due and it becomes too uncomfortable to put off any longer. Unfortunately, waiting until the last moment means that you can rarely put as much time and effort into your course-work as you need to in order to reach your potential.
✓ You want to overcome your anxiety of travelling away from home by facing your fear directly. And yet, each time you try to travel farther on the train, you become anxious, and think ‘This is so horrible, I can’t stand it’, and quickly return home, which reinforces your fear rather than helping you experience travel as less threatening.
The best way to overcome low frustration tolerance is to foster an alternative attitude of high frustration tolerance. You can achieve this way of thinking by trying the following:
✓ Pushing yourself to do things that are uncomfortable or unpleasant.
For example, you can train yourself to work on assignments even if you aren’t in a good mood, because the end result of finishing work in good time, and to a good standard, outweighs the hassle of doing something you find tedious.
✓ Giving yourself messages that emphasize your ability to withstand pain. To combat a fear of travel, you can remind yourself that feeling anxious is really unpleasant, but you can stand it. Ask yourself whether, in the past, you’ve ever withstood the feelings you’re saying you presently ‘can’t stand’.
Telling yourself you can’t stand something has two effects. First, it leads you to focus more on the discomfort you’re experiencing. Second, it leads you to underestimate your ability to cope with discomfort. Many things can be difficult to tolerate, but rating them as ‘intolerable’ often makes situations seem more daunting than they really are.
CBT 12
Personalizing: Removing Yourself from the Center of the Universe
Personalizing involves interpreting events as being related to you personally and overlooking other factors. This can lead to emotional difficulties, such as feeling hurt easily or feeling unnecessarily guilty.
Spotting Errors in Your Thinking
Here are some examples of personalizing:
✓ You may tend to feel guilty if you know a friend is upset and you can’t make him feel better. You think, ‘If I was really a good friend, I’d be able to cheer him up. I’m obviously letting him down.’
✓ You feel hurt when a friend you meet in a shop leaves quickly after saying only a hurried ‘hello’. You think, ‘He was obviously trying to avoid talking to me. I must have offended him somehow.’
You can tackle personalizing by considering alternative explanations that don’t revolve around you. Think about the following examples:
✓ Imagine what else may contribute to the outcome you’re assuming personal responsibility for. Your friend may have lost his job or be suffering from depression. Despite your best efforts to cheer him up, these factors are outside your control.
✓ Consider why people may be responding to you in a certain way.
Don’t jump to the conclusion that someone’s response relates directly to you. For example, your friend may be having a difficult day or be in a big hurry – he may even feel sorry for not stopping to talk to you.
Because you really aren’t the center of the universe, look for explanations of events that have little or nothing to do with you.
APPENDIX E – Collected data
All | Females | Males | |
ISTJ | 33 | 10 | 23 |
ISFJ | 39 | 28 | 11 |
INFJ | 4 | 3 | 1 |
INTJ | 5 | 1 | 4 |
ISTP | 15 | 3 | 12 |
ISFP | 25 | 15 | 10 |
INFP | 12 | 7 | 5 |
INTP | 9 | 3 | 6 |
ESTP | 12 | 5 | 7 |
ESFP | 24 | 15 | 9 |
ENFP | 23 | 14 | 9 |
ENTP | 9 | 4 | 5 |
ESTJ | 24 | 9 | 15 |
ESFJ | 35 | 25 | 10 |
ENFJ | 7 | 5 | 2 |
ENTJ | 5 | 2 | 3 |
Totals | 281 | 149 | 132 |
APPENDIX F – Statistical Data Analysis
Analysis of Percentages Comparing the Abbreviated MBTI with the Full MBTI Assessment for ‘All’, ‘Female’, and ‘Male’ categories
Briggs-Myers, I., McCaulley, M. H., Quenk, N. L., & Hammer, A. L. (2003). MBTI manual. A guide to the development and use of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. 3rd Edition. Menlo Park, CA: CPP Inc.
All subjects
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
General population | Subjects | |
Mean | 6.26625 | 6.25 |
Variance | 16.2131583 | 16.8516736 |
Observations | 16 | 16 |
df | 15 | 15 |
F | 0.96210968 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.47067912 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.41606907 | |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (General) = variance (Study)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
ISTJ | -0.144 |
ISFJ | -0.079 |
INFJ | 0.037 |
INTJ | 0.321 |
ISTP | 0.062 |
ISFP | -0.097 |
INFP | 0.130 |
INTP | 0.097 |
ESTP | 0.030 |
ESFP | -0.041 |
ENFP | -0.085 |
ENTP | -0.003 |
ESTJ | 0.159 |
ESFJ | -0.156 |
ENFJ | 0.009 |
ENTJ | 0.021 |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9718
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.914
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Mean-matched pairs T-test
Claim: µ(population) – µ(study) = 0
Sample size, n: 16
Difference Mean, d: 0.01625
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.1234436
Test Statistic, t: 0.5266 Critical t: ±2.9467
P-Value: 0.6062
99% Confidence interval:
-0.0746882 < µd < 0.1071882
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim that the mean difference between the two lists is 0.
Correlation and Regression test
Sample size, n: 16 Degrees of freedom: 14
Correlation Results:
Correlation coeff, r: 0.9997304 Critical r: ±0.6225903
P-value (two-tailed): 0.000
Reject the null hypothesis – Sample provides evidence to support linear correlation
Regression Results:
Y= b0 + b1x: Y Intercept, b0: -0.1382738 Slope, b1: 1.019473
Total Variation: 252.8976 Explained Variation: 252.7612
Unexplained Variation: 0.1363535
Standard Error: 0.0986891
Coeff of Det, R^2: 0.9994608
Conclusion:
The results of both the T-test (mean difference =0) and the correlation graph (approximately a 45 degree line), indicate that the percentages of individuals in the sample tested by means of the spreadsheet (Appendix C) matches well with the general population measured by the full MBTI test.
Percentage of females in each MBTI category in the general population vs study
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Expected | Observed | |
Mean | 6.25 | 6.25 |
Variance | 29.8916715 | 31.88 |
Observations | 16 | 16 |
df | 15 | 15 |
F | 0.93763085 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.45119692 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.41606907 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Expected) = variance (Observed)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
ISTJ | 0.189 |
ISFJ | 0.608 |
INFJ | -0.413 |
INTJ | 0.129 |
ISTP | 0.387 |
ISFP | -0.167 |
INFP | -0.098 |
INTP | -0.213 |
ESTP | -0.356 |
ESFP | 0.033 |
ENFP | 0.304 |
ENTP | -0.285 |
ESTJ | 0.260 |
ESFJ | 0.121 |
ENFJ | -0.056 |
ENTJ | -0.442 |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9901
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.914
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Mean-matched pairs T-test
Claim: µ(population) – µ(study) = 0
Sample size, n: 16
Difference Mean, d: -2.914335e-16 Difference St Dev, sd: 0.3062025
Test Statistic, t: -0.0000 Critical t: ±2.9467
P-Value: 1.0000
99% Confidence interval:
-0.2255726 < µd < 0.2255726
Fail to reject the null hypothesis – Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim the mean difference between the two lists of data is 0.
Correlation and Regression Test
Sample size, n: 16 Degrees of freedom: 14
Correlation Results:
Correlation coeff, r: 0.9989947 Critical r: ±0.4973094
P-value (two-tailed): 0.000
Reject the null hypothesis – Sample provides evidence to support linear correlation
Regression Results:
Y= b0 + b1x: Y Intercept, b0: 0.2031185 Slope, b1: 0.967501
Total Variation: 448.5244 Explained Variation: 447.6231
Unexplained Variation: 0.9013337
Standard Error: 0.2537341 Coeff of Det, R^2: 0.9979904
Conclusion:
The results of both the T-test (mean difference =0) and the correlation graph (approximately a 45 degree line), indicate that the percentages of females in the sample tested by means of the spreadsheet (Appendix C) matches well with the general population of females measured by the full MBTI test.
Percentage of males in each category in the general population vs study
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Expected | Observed | |
Mean | 6.25 | 6.25 |
Variance | 14.632 | 17.48546067 |
Observations | 16 | 16 |
df | 15 | 15 |
F | 0.83680952 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.36727296 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.41606907 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Expected) = variance (Observed)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
ISTJ | -1.024 |
ISFJ | -0.233 |
INFJ | 0.542 |
INTJ | 0.270 |
ISTP | -0.591 |
ISFP | 0.024 |
INFP | 0.312 |
INTP | 0.255 |
ESTP | 0.297 |
ESFP | 0.082 |
ENFP | -0.418 |
ENTP | 0.212 |
ESTJ | -0.164 |
ESFJ | -0.076 |
ENFJ | 0.085 |
ENTJ | 0.427 |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9557
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.914
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 1
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Mean-matched pairs T-test
Claim: µ(population) – µ(study) = 0
Sample size, n: 16
Difference Mean, d: -0.000625 Difference St Dev, sd: 0.408664
Test Statistic, t: -0.0061 Critical t: ±2.9467
P-Value: 0.9952
99% Confidence interval: -0.3016786 < µd < 0.3004286
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim that the mean difference between the two lists is 0.
Correlation and Regression
Sample size, n: 16 Degrees of freedom: 14
Correlation Results: Correlation coeff, r: 0.9987131 Critical r: ±0.6225903
P-value (two-tailed): 0.000
Reject the null hypothesis
Sample provides evidence to support linear correlation
Regression Results:
Y= b0 + b1x: Y Intercept, b0: -0.5702271 Slope, b1: 1.091336
Total Variation: 262.0781 Explained Variation: 261.404
Unexplained Variation: 0.6741198
Standard Error: 0.2194343 Coeff of Det, R^2: 0.9974278
Conclusion:
The results of both the T-test (mean difference =0) and the correlation graph (approximately a 45 degree line), indicate that the percentages of males in the sample tested by means of the spreadsheet (Appendix C) matches well with the general population of males measured by the full MBTI test.
ANOVA test on normalized data differences to establish differential effects from CBT concept exposure for the ‘All subjects’ category
‘Before mean’ minus ‘After mean’ for each group:
ISTJ | ISFJ | INFJ | INTJ | ISTP | ISFP | INFP | INTP |
-0.122 | -0.010 | -0.012 | -0.108 | -0.040 | -0.015 | -0.017 | 0.012 |
-0.182 | -0.050 | -0.017 | -0.163 | -0.032 | -0.013 | 0.009 | 0.022 |
-0.140 | 0.014 | 0.023 | -0.176 | 0.008 | -0.025 | -0.033 | -0.023 |
-0.161 | -0.037 | 0.014 | -0.169 | 0.025 | -0.004 | 0.001 | -0.032 |
-0.093 | 0.005 | -0.033 | -0.080 | 0.012 | -0.027 | -0.019 | 0.014 |
-0.123 | -0.013 | 0.021 | -0.177 | -0.050 | -0.005 | -0.008 | -0.014 |
ESTP | ESFP | ENFP | ENTP | ESTJ | ESFJ | ENFJ | ENTJ |
-0.022 | -0.013 | 0.019 | 0.009 | -0.008 | 0.028 | 0.003 | -0.042 |
-0.028 | 0.013 | 0.002 | -0.045 | -0.028 | -0.011 | 0.023 | -0.035 |
0.001 | 0.019 | 0.037 | 0.015 | -0.009 | -0.012 | -0.015 | 0.025 |
-0.003 | -0.017 | -0.003 | -0.021 | -0.010 | -0.039 | -0.019 | -0.044 |
-0.020 | -0.018 | -0.024 | 0.005 | -0.024 | -0.030 | -0.018 | -0.003 |
0.014 | -0.035 | -0.005 | -0.077 | 0.023 | 0.021 | -0.024 | -0.019 |
Bartlett’s Test to confirm homoscedasticity
Significance level (%): 5 | |||||||
Summary statistics: | |||||||
All | Minimum | Maximum | Mean | ||||
ISTJ | -0.182 | -0.093 | -0.137 | ||||
ISFJ | -0.05 | 0.014 | -0.015 | ||||
INFJ | -0.033 | 0.023 | -0.001 | ||||
INTJ | -0.177 | -0.08 | -0.146 | ||||
ISTP | -0.05 | 0.025 | -0.013 | ||||
ISFP | -0.027 | -0.004 | -0.015 | ||||
INFP | -0.033 | 0.009 | -0.011 | ||||
INTP | -0.032 | 0.022 | -0.004 | ||||
ESTP | -0.028 | 0.014 | -0.01 | ||||
ESFP | -0.035 | 0.019 | -0.009 | ||||
ENFP | -0.024 | 0.037 | 0.004 | ||||
ENTP | -0.077 | 0.015 | -0.019 | ||||
ESTJ | -0.028 | 0.023 | -0.009 | ||||
ESFJ | -0.039 | 0.028 | -0.007 | ||||
ENFJ | -0.024 | 0.023 | -0.008 | ||||
ENTJ | -0.044 | 0.025 | -0.02 | ||||
Chi-square (Observed value) | 16.647 | ||||||
Chi-square (Critical value) | 24.996 | ||||||
DF | 15 | ||||||
p-value (one-tailed) | 0.34 | ||||||
alpha | 0.05 | ||||||
Test interpretation: | |||||||
H0: The variances are identical. | |||||||
Ha: At least one of the variances is different from another. | |||||||
As the computed p-value is greater than the significance level alpha=0.05, one cannot reject the null hypothesis H0. | |||||||
ANOVA results:
Source: DF: SS: MS: Test Stat, F: Critical F: P-Value:
Treatment: 15 0.185246 0.01235 19.444022 1.79322 0.000
Error: 80 0.050812 0.000635
Total: 95 0.236058
Reject the null hypothesis
Reject equality of means
Conclusion: There are differential effects within the 16 groups in the ‘All subjects’ category.
ANOVA test on normalized data differences to establish differential effects from CBT concept exposure for the ‘Females’ category
‘Before mean’ minus ‘After mean’ for each group:
ISTJ | ISFJ | INFJ | INTJ | ISTP | ISFP | INFP | INTP |
-0.114 | 0.016 | 0.016 | -0.095 | -0.032 | 0.011 | 0.020 | 0.032 |
-0.171 | -0.008 | 0.010 | -0.143 | 0.010 | 0.010 | 0.016 | 0.038 |
-0.151 | 0.065 | 0.010 | -0.171 | 0.038 | -0.042 | -0.057 | 0.000 |
-0.200 | -0.039 | 0.048 | -0.229 | 0.038 | -0.032 | 0.004 | -0.019 |
-0.081 | -0.027 | -0.014 | -0.083 | 0.007 | -0.017 | -0.015 | 0.014 |
-0.146 | -0.022 | -0.012 | -0.214 | 0.012 | -0.033 | -0.010 | 0.024 |
ESTP | ESFP | ENFP | ENTP | ESTJ | ESFJ | ENFJ | ENTJ |
-0.043 | -0.013 | 0.032 | -0.024 | 0.013 | 0.045 | -0.019 | -0.048 |
0.029 | 0.008 | 0.035 | -0.043 | 0.016 | 0.022 | 0.011 | 0.014 |
-0.080 | 0.027 | 0.057 | -0.029 | 0.044 | 0.027 | -0.017 | -0.029 |
-0.029 | 0.040 | 0.047 | -0.029 | -0.016 | 0.022 | -0.029 | -0.057 |
0.008 | -0.032 | -0.025 | 0.016 | -0.049 | -0.051 | 0.038 | 0.021 |
0.000 | -0.017 | -0.061 | -0.107 | 0.032 | -0.011 | 0.043 | -0.036 |
Bartlett’s Test to confirm homoscedasticity FemalesMinimumMaximumMeanStd. deviationISTJ-0.186-0.098-0.1340.031ISFJ-0.0680.019-0.0210.029INFJ-0.0420.036-0.0050.032INTJ-0.179-0.078-0.1410.039ISTP-0.0770.019-0.0240.039ISFP-0.0310.009-0.0140.017INFP-0.0330.006-0.0130.015INTP-0.0380.014-0.0120.025ESTP-0.0530.037-0.0060.034ESFP-0.0440.016-0.0130.026ENFP-0.0250.0290.0000.023ENTP-0.0640.034-0.0120.039ESTJ-0.0480.019-0.0170.023ESFJ-0.0660.036-0.0140.037ENFJ-0.0540.029-0.0140.031ENTJ-0.0570.048-0.0190.037 | Bartlett’s Test to confirm homoscedasticity | Bartlett’s Test to confirm homoscedasticity | Bartlett’s Test to confirm homoscedasticity | |||||
Chi-square (Observed value) | 16.647 | |||||||
Chi-square (Critical value) | 24.996 | |||||||
DF | 15 | |||||||
p-value (one-tailed) | 0.340 | |||||||
alpha | 0.05 | |||||||
Test interpretation: | ||||||||
H0: The variances are identical. | ||||||||
Ha: At least one of the variances is different from another. | ||||||||
As the computed p-value is greater than the significance level alpha=0.05, one cannot reject the null hypothesis H0. ANOVA results:Source: DF: SS: MS: Test Stat, F: Critical F: P-Value: Treatment: 15 0.248781 0.016585 13.110759 1.79322 0.000 Error: 80 0.101202 0.001265 Total: 95 0.349983 Reject the null hypothesisReject equality of means | ||||||||
Conclusion: There are differential effects within the 16 groups in the ‘Females’ category.
ANOVA test on normalized data differences to establish differential effects from CBT concept exposure for the ‘Males’ category.
ISTJ | ISFJ | INFJ | INTJ | ISTP | ISFP | INFP | INTP |
-0.125 | -0.022 | -0.024 | -0.113 | -0.044 | -0.026 | -0.033 | 0.004 |
-0.186 | -0.068 | -0.029 | -0.171 | -0.050 | -0.023 | 0.006 | 0.014 |
-0.135 | -0.008 | 0.029 | -0.179 | -0.005 | -0.017 | -0.023 | -0.033 |
-0.144 | -0.036 | 0.000 | -0.143 | 0.019 | 0.009 | 0.000 | -0.038 |
-0.098 | 0.019 | -0.042 | -0.078 | 0.014 | -0.031 | -0.021 | 0.014 |
-0.113 | -0.010 | 0.036 | -0.161 | -0.077 | 0.007 | -0.007 | -0.030 |
ESTP | ESFP | ENFP | ENTP | ESTJ | ESFJ | ENFJ | ENTJ |
-0.014 | -0.013 | 0.013 | 0.024 | -0.017 | 0.021 | 0.012 | -0.040 |
-0.053 | 0.016 | -0.013 | -0.046 | -0.048 | -0.026 | 0.029 | -0.057 |
0.037 | 0.016 | 0.029 | 0.034 | -0.032 | -0.029 | -0.014 | 0.048 |
0.008 | -0.041 | -0.025 | -0.017 | -0.008 | -0.066 | -0.014 | -0.038 |
-0.033 | -0.012 | -0.023 | 0.000 | -0.014 | -0.021 | -0.042 | -0.014 |
0.020 | -0.044 | 0.020 | -0.064 | 0.019 | 0.036 | -0.054 | -0.012 |
Bartlett’s Test to confirm homoscedasticity | Bartlett’s Test to confirm homoscedasticity | Bartlett’s Test to confirm homoscedasticity | Bartlett’s Test to confirm homoscedasticity | ||
Males | Minimum | Maximum | Mean | Std. deviation | |
ISTJ | -0.186 | -0.098 | -0.134 | 0.031 | |
ISFJ | -0.068 | 0.019 | -0.021 | 0.029 | |
INFJ | -0.042 | 0.036 | -0.005 | 0.032 | |
INTJ | -0.179 | -0.078 | -0.141 | 0.039 | |
ISTP | -0.077 | 0.019 | -0.024 | 0.039 | |
ISFP | -0.031 | 0.009 | -0.014 | 0.017 | |
INFP | -0.033 | 0.006 | -0.013 | 0.015 | |
INTP | -0.038 | 0.014 | -0.012 | 0.025 | |
ESTP | -0.053 | 0.037 | -0.006 | 0.034 | |
ESFP | -0.044 | 0.016 | -0.013 | 0.026 | |
ENFP | -0.025 | 0.029 | 0.000 | 0.023 | |
ENTP | -0.064 | 0.034 | -0.012 | 0.039 | |
ESTJ | -0.048 | 0.019 | -0.017 | 0.023 | |
ESFJ | -0.066 | 0.036 | -0.014 | 0.037 | |
ENFJ | -0.054 | 0.029 | -0.014 | 0.031 | |
ENTJ | -0.057 | 0.048 | -0.019 | 0.037 |
Bartlett’s test / Two-tailed test (ISTJ): | ||||||||
Chi-square (Observed value) | 9.805 | |||||||
Chi-square (Critical value) | 24.996 | |||||||
DF | 15 | |||||||
p-value (one-tailed) | 0.832 | |||||||
alpha | 0.05 | |||||||
Test interpretation: | ||||||||
H0: The variances are identical. | ||||||||
Ha: At least one of the variances is different from another. | ||||||||
As the computed p-value is greater than the significance level alpha=0.05, one cannot reject the null hypothesis H0. | ||||||||
ANOVA results:
Source: DF: SS: MS: Test Stat, F: Critical F: P-Value:
Treatment: 15 0.165298 0.01102 11.71049 1.79322 0.000
Error: 80 0.075282 0.000941
Total: 95 0.24058
Reject the null hypothesis
Reject equality of means
Conclusion: There are differential effects within the 16 groups in the ‘Males’ category.
Tests for homoscedasticity, data normality and differential effects from CBT concept exposure for each of the 16 MBTI categories for ‘All subjects’, ‘Females’, and ‘Males’
ISTJ – All subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 29.52 | 34.64 | / 42 | 0.70 | 0.82 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 27.82 | 34.18 | /35 | 0.79 | 0.98 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 29.27 | 34.18 | /35 | 0.84 | 0.98 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 28.76 | 34.39 | /35 | 0.82 | 0.98 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 38.85 | 43.30 | /48 | 0.81 | 0.90 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.36 | 26.82 | /28 | 0.83 | 0.96 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.79833333 | 0.936666667 |
Variance | 0.00261667 | 0.004226667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.61908517 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.30578095 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.122 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.182 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.140 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.161 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.093 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.123 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9879
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) < 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.1383333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0343026
Test Statistic, t: -9.8782
Critical t: -2.0150
P-Value: 0.0001
90% Confidence interval:
-0.166552 < µd < -0.1101147
Reject the null hypothesis
Sample provides evidence to support the claim
Conclusion:
Reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure minus after CBT concept exposure is less than zero.
The results show that there was significant improvement in the ISTJ ‘All Subjects’ category.
ISTJ – Female subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 30.80 | 35.60 | / 42 | 0.73 | 0.85 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 26.70 | 32.70 | /35 | 0.76 | 0.93 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 29.70 | 35.00 | /35 | 0.85 | 1.00 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 28.10 | 35.10 | /35 | 0.80 | 1.00 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 38.80 | 42.70 | /48 | 0.81 | 0.89 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.00 | 27.10 | /28 | 0.82 | 0.97 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.795 | 0.94 |
Variance | 0.00187 | 0.00376 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.49734043 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.23082888 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.114 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.171 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.151 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.200 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.081 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.146 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9907
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) < 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.145
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0413521
Test Statistic, t: -8.5891
Critical t: -2.0150
P-Value: 0.0002
90% Confidence interval:
-0.1790179 < µd < -0.1109821
Reject the null hypothesis
Sample provides evidence to support the claim
Conclusion:
Reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure minus after CBT concept exposure is less than zero.
The results show that there was significant improvement in the ISTJ ‘Female Subjects’ category.
ISTJ – Male Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 28.96 | 34.22 | / 42 | 0.69 | 0.81 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 28.30 | 34.83 | /35 | 0.81 | 1.00 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 29.09 | 33.83 | /35 | 0.83 | 0.97 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.04 | 34.09 | /35 | 0.83 | 0.97 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 38.87 | 43.57 | /48 | 0.81 | 0.91 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.52 | 26.70 | /28 | 0.84 | 0.95 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.80166667 | 0.935 |
Variance | 0.00313667 | 0.00463 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.6774658 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.33979224 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.125 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.186 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.135 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.144 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.098 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.113 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9678
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) < 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.1333333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0320416
Test Statistic, t: -10.1929
Critical t: -2.0150
P-Value: 0.0001
90% Confidence interval:
-0.159692 < µd < -0.1069746
Reject the null hypothesis
Sample provides evidence to support the claim
Conclusion:
Reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure minus after CBT concept exposure is less than zero.
The results show that there was significant improvement in the ISTJ ‘Male Subjects’ category.
ISFJ All subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 32.56 | 32.99 | / 42 | 0.78 | 0.79 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 26.52 | 28.26 | /35 | 0.76 | 0.81 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 33.15 | 32.65 | /35 | 0.95 | 0.93 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 27.90 | 29.20 | /35 | 0.80 | 0.83 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 37.09 | 36.84 | /48 | 0.77 | 0.77 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 20.80 | 21.18 | /28 | 0.74 | 0.76 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.8 | 0.815 |
Variance | 0.0058 | 0.00383 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 1.51436031 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.32996683 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.010 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.050 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.014 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.037 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
0.005 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.013 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.981
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.015
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0242899
Test Statistic, t: -1.5127
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.1908
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0404907 < µd < 0.0104907
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ISFJ ‘All Subjects’ category.
ISFJ Female Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 30.71 | 30.04 | / 42 | 0.73 | 0.72 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.39 | 29.68 | /35 | 0.84 | 0.85 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 35.39 | 33.11 | /35 | 1.01 | 0.95 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 27.68 | 29.04 | /35 | 0.79 | 0.83 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 40.00 | 41.29 | /48 | 0.83 | 0.86 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.07 | 23.68 | /28 | 0.82 | 0.85 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.83666667 | 0.843333333 |
Variance | 0.00878667 | 0.005426667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 1.61916462 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.30489889 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
0.016 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.008 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.065 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.039 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.027 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.022 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9397
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0066667
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.037238
Test Statistic, t: -0.4385
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.6793
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0457455 < µd < 0.0324122
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ISFJ ‘Female Subjects’ category.
ISFJ Male Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 33.36 | 34.27 | / 42 | 0.79 | 0.82 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 25.27 | 27.64 | /35 | 0.72 | 0.79 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 32.18 | 32.45 | /35 | 0.92 | 0.93 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 28.00 | 29.27 | /35 | 0.80 | 0.84 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 35.82 | 34.91 | /48 | 0.75 | 0.73 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 19.82 | 20.09 | /28 | 0.71 | 0.72 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.78166667 | 0.805 |
Variance | 0.00589667 | 0.00603 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.97788834 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.4905112 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.022 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.068 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.008 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.036 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
0.019 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.010 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9796
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0233333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0307679
Test Statistic, t: -1.8576
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.1223
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0556223 < µd < 0.0089556
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ISFJ ‘Male Subjects’ category.
INFJ All Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 30.00 | 30.49 | / 42 | 0.71 | 0.73 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.09 | 29.69 | /35 | 0.83 | 0.85 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 31.10 | 30.30 | /35 | 0.89 | 0.87 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 30.30 | 29.80 | /35 | 0.87 | 0.85 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 36.40 | 38.00 | /48 | 0.76 | 0.79 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 24.99 | 24.39 | /28 | 0.89 | 0.87 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.825 | 0.826666667 |
Variance | 0.00559 | 0.003106667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 1.79935622 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.26737734 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.012 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.017 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.023 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.014 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.033 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
0.021 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9561
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0016667
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0240139
Test Statistic, t: -0.1700
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.8717
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0268677 < µd < 0.0235343
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the INFJ ‘All Subjects’ category.
INFJ – Female Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 30.00 | 29.33 | / 42 | 0.71 | 0.70 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 27.00 | 26.67 | /35 | 0.77 | 0.76 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 31.33 | 31.00 | /35 | 0.90 | 0.89 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 31.00 | 29.33 | /35 | 0.89 | 0.84 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 37.33 | 38.00 | /48 | 0.78 | 0.79 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 22.67 | 23.00 | /28 | 0.81 | 0.82 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.81 | 0.8 |
Variance | 0.0054 | 0.00436 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 1.23853211 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.41006391 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
0.016 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.010 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.010 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.048 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.014 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.012 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9445
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: 0.01
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0219089
Test Statistic, t: 1.1180
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.3144
95% Confidence interval:
-0.012992 < µd < 0.032992
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the INFJ ‘Female Subjects’ category.
INFJ – Male Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 30.00 | 31.00 | / 42 | 0.71 | 0.74 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 30.00 | 31.00 | /35 | 0.86 | 0.89 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 31.00 | 30.00 | /35 | 0.89 | 0.86 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 30.00 | 30.00 | /35 | 0.86 | 0.86 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 36.00 | 38.00 | /48 | 0.75 | 0.79 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 26.00 | 25.00 | /28 | 0.93 | 0.89 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.83333333 | 0.838333333 |
Variance | 0.00722667 | 0.003656667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 1.976299 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.23629904 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.024 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.029 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.029 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.000 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.042 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
0.036 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9653
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.005
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0339116
Test Statistic, t: -0.3612
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.7327
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0405881 < µd < 0.0305881
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence do not support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is different from zero.
The results show that there was no significant change in the INFJ ‘Male Subjects’ category.
INTJ – All Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 28.83 | 33.35 | / 42 | 0.69 | 0.79 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 28.17 | 33.87 | /35 | 0.80 | 0.97 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 27.87 | 34.05 | /35 | 0.80 | 0.97 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 27.05 | 32.95 | /35 | 0.77 | 0.94 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 37.52 | 41.35 | /48 | 0.78 | 0.86 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 21.70 | 26.65 | /28 | 0.77 | 0.95 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.76833333 | 0.913333333 |
Variance | 0.00165667 | 0.005306667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.31218593 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.11358262 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.108 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.163 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.176 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.169 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.080 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.177 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.8977
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) < 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.145
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0432435
Test Statistic, t: -8.2134
Critical t: -2.0150
P-Value: 0.0002
90% Confidence interval:
-0.1805738 < µd < -0.1094262
Reject the null hypothesis
Sample provides evidence to support the claim
Conclusion:
Reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure minus after CBT concept exposure is less than zero.
The results show that there was significant improvement in the INTJ ‘All Subjects’ category.
INTJ – Female Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 29.00 | 33.00 | / 42 | 0.69 | 0.79 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 28.00 | 33.00 | /35 | 0.80 | 0.94 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 27.00 | 33.00 | /35 | 0.77 | 0.94 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 26.00 | 34.00 | /35 | 0.74 | 0.97 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 37.00 | 41.00 | /48 | 0.77 | 0.85 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 21.00 | 27.00 | /28 | 0.75 | 0.96 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.75333333 | 0.908333333 |
Variance | 0.00138667 | 0.005176667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.26786864 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.08729761 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.095 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.143 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.171 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.229 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.083 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.214 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9765
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) < 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.155
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0595819
Test Statistic, t: -6.3723
Critical t: -2.0150
P-Value: 0.0007
90% Confidence interval:
-0.2040144 < µd < -0.1059856
Reject the null hypothesis
Sample provides evidence to support the claim
Conclusion:
Reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure minus after CBT concept exposure is less than zero.
The results show that there was significant improvement in the INTJ ‘Female Subjects’ category.
INTJ – Male Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 28.75 | 33.50 | / 42 | 0.68 | 0.80 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 28.25 | 34.25 | /35 | 0.81 | 0.98 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 28.25 | 34.50 | /35 | 0.81 | 0.99 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 27.50 | 32.50 | /35 | 0.79 | 0.93 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 37.75 | 41.50 | /48 | 0.79 | 0.86 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 22.00 | 26.50 | /28 | 0.79 | 0.95 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.77833333 | 0.918333333 |
Variance | 0.00241667 | 0.005496667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.4396604 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.19406773 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.113 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.171 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.179 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.143 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.078 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.161 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9606
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) < 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.14
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0404969
Test Statistic, t: -8.4680
Critical t: -2.0150
P-Value: 0.0002
90% Confidence interval:
-0.1733144 < µd < -0.1066856
Reject the null hypothesis
Sample provides evidence to support the claim
Conclusion:
Reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure minus after CBT concept exposure is less than zero.
The results show that there was significant improvement in the INTJ ‘Male Subjects’ category.
ISTP – All Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 33.24 | 34.92 | / 42 | 0.79 | 0.83 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 28.10 | 29.22 | /35 | 0.80 | 0.83 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 29.96 | 29.67 | /35 | 0.86 | 0.85 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.41 | 28.54 | /35 | 0.84 | 0.82 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 41.00 | 40.44 | /48 | 0.85 | 0.84 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 22.66 | 24.07 | /28 | 0.81 | 0.86 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.825 | 0.838333333 |
Variance | 0.00083 | 0.000216667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 3.83076923 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.08339813 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.040 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.032 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.008 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.025 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
0.012 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.050 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9535
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0133333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0301109
Test Statistic, t: -1.0847
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.3276
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0449328 < µd < 0.0182661
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ISTP ‘All Subjects’ category.
ISTP – Female Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 30.33 | 31.67 | / 42 | 0.72 | 0.75 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 27.00 | 26.67 | /35 | 0.77 | 0.76 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 29.67 | 28.33 | /35 | 0.85 | 0.81 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 26.33 | 25.00 | /35 | 0.75 | 0.71 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.67 | 39.33 | /48 | 0.83 | 0.82 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 25.33 | 25.00 | /28 | 0.90 | 0.89 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.80333333 | 0.79 |
Variance | 0.00462667 | 0.00404 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 1.14521452 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.44267254 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.032 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.010 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.038 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.038 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
0.007 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
0.012 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9331
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: 0.0133333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0258199
Test Statistic, t: 1.2649
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.2617
95% Confidence interval:
-0.013763 < µd < 0.0404296
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ISTP ‘Female Subjects’ category.
ISTP – Male Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 34.50 | 36.33 | / 42 | 0.82 | 0.87 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 28.58 | 30.33 | /35 | 0.82 | 0.87 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 30.08 | 30.25 | /35 | 0.86 | 0.86 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 30.75 | 30.08 | /35 | 0.88 | 0.86 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 41.58 | 40.92 | /48 | 0.87 | 0.85 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 21.50 | 23.67 | /28 | 0.77 | 0.85 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.83666667 | 0.86 |
Variance | 0.00170667 | 8E-05 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 21.3333333 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.0021951 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p < .05, reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
t-Test: Two-Sample Assuming Unequal Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.83666667 | 0.86 |
Variance | 0.00170667 | 8E-05 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
Hypothesized Mean Difference | 0 | |
df | 5 | |
t Stat | -1.352168 | |
P(T<=t) one-tail | 0.11713087 | |
t Critical one-tail | 2.01504837 | |
P(T<=t) two-tail | 0.23426174 | |
t Critical two-tail | 2.57058183 |
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.044 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.050 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.005 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.019 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
0.014 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.077 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9677
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0233333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.042269
Test Statistic, t: -1.3522
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.2343
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0676919 < µd < 0.0210252
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence do not support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is different from zero.
The results show that there was no significant change in the ISTP ‘Male Subjects’ category.
ISFP – All Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 31.80 | 32.42 | / 42 | 0.76 | 0.77 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.61 | 30.07 | /35 | 0.85 | 0.86 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 27.37 | 28.24 | /35 | 0.78 | 0.81 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 27.59 | 27.73 | /35 | 0.79 | 0.79 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 38.11 | 39.40 | /48 | 0.79 | 0.82 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 24.59 | 24.73 | /28 | 0.88 | 0.88 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.80833333 | 0.821666667 |
Variance | 0.00213667 | 0.001736667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 1.2303263 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.41280819 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.015 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.013 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.025 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.004 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.027 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.005 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.964
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0133333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0136626
Test Statistic, t: -2.3905
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.0624
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0276713 < µd < 0.0010047
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ISFP ‘All Subjects’ category.
ISFP – Female Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 33.40 | 32.93 | / 42 | 0.80 | 0.78 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.87 | 29.53 | /35 | 0.85 | 0.84 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 28.47 | 29.93 | /35 | 0.81 | 0.86 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 27.80 | 28.93 | /35 | 0.79 | 0.83 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 40.67 | 41.47 | /48 | 0.85 | 0.86 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.87 | 24.80 | /28 | 0.85 | 0.89 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.825 | 0.843333333 |
Variance | 0.00079 | 0.001386667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.56971154 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.2759627 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
0.011 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.010 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.042 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.032 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.017 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.033 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.941
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0183333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0292689
Test Statistic, t: -1.5343
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.1855
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0490491 < µd < 0.0123824
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ISFP ‘Female Subjects’ category.
ISFP – Male Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 31.10 | 32.20 | / 42 | 0.74 | 0.77 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.50 | 30.30 | /35 | 0.84 | 0.87 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 26.90 | 27.50 | /35 | 0.77 | 0.79 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 27.50 | 27.20 | /35 | 0.79 | 0.78 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 37.00 | 38.50 | /48 | 0.77 | 0.80 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 24.90 | 24.70 | /28 | 0.89 | 0.88 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.8 | 0.815 |
Variance | 0.00304 | 0.00227 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 1.33920705 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.37820222 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.026 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.023 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.017 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.009 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.031 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
0.007 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9319
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.015
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0197484
Test Statistic, t: -1.8605
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.1219
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0357247 < µd < 0.0057247
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence do not support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is different from zero.
The results show that there was no significant change in the ISFP ‘Male Subjects’ category.
INFP – All Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 32.36 | 33.07 | / 42 | 0.77 | 0.79 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.28 | 28.97 | /35 | 0.84 | 0.83 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 28.64 | 29.80 | /35 | 0.82 | 0.85 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 31.02 | 30.98 | /35 | 0.89 | 0.89 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 38.78 | 39.69 | /48 | 0.81 | 0.83 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 24.62 | 24.85 | /28 | 0.88 | 0.89 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.835 | 0.846666667 |
Variance | 0.00203 | 0.001506667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 1.34734513 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.37576669 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.017 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.009 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.033 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.001 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.019 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.008 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9922
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0116667
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0147196
Test Statistic, t: -1.9415
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.1099
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0271139 < µd < 0.0037806
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the INFP ‘All Subjects’ category.
INFP – Female Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 32.71 | 31.86 | / 42 | 0.78 | 0.76 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 30.86 | 30.29 | /35 | 0.88 | 0.87 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 26.43 | 28.43 | /35 | 0.76 | 0.81 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 30.14 | 30.00 | /35 | 0.86 | 0.86 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 40.57 | 41.29 | /48 | 0.85 | 0.86 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.29 | 23.57 | /28 | 0.83 | 0.84 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.82666667 | 0.833333333 |
Variance | 0.00222667 | 0.001746667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 1.27480916 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.39820383 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
0.020 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.016 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.057 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.004 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.015 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.010 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9433
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0066667
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0242212
Test Statistic, t: -0.6742
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.5301
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0320852 < µd < 0.0187519
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the INFP ‘Female Subjects’ category.
INFP – Male Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 32.20 | 33.60 | / 42 | 0.77 | 0.80 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 28.60 | 28.40 | /35 | 0.82 | 0.81 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 29.60 | 30.40 | /35 | 0.85 | 0.87 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 31.40 | 31.40 | /35 | 0.90 | 0.90 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 38.00 | 39.00 | /48 | 0.79 | 0.81 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 25.20 | 25.40 | /28 | 0.90 | 0.91 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.83833333 | 0.85 |
Variance | 0.00301667 | 0.00244 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 1.2363388 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.41079517 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.033 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.006 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.023 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.000 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.021 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.007 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.982
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0116667
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0147196
Test Statistic, t: -1.9415
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.1099
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0271139 < µd < 0.0037806
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence do not support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is different from zero.
The results show that there was no significant change in the INFP ‘Male Subjects’ category.
INTP – All Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 29.43 | 28.91 | / 42 | 0.70 | 0.69 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.68 | 28.93 | /35 | 0.85 | 0.83 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 28.77 | 29.58 | /35 | 0.82 | 0.85 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.77 | 30.90 | /35 | 0.85 | 0.88 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 38.15 | 37.48 | /48 | 0.79 | 0.78 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 22.41 | 22.79 | /28 | 0.80 | 0.81 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.80166667 | 0.806666667 |
Variance | 0.00309667 | 0.004426667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.69954819 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.35227544 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
0.012 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.022 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.023 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.032 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
0.014 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.014 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9597
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.005
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0216795
Test Statistic, t: -0.5649
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.5965
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0277512 < µd < 0.0177512
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the INTP ‘All Subjects’ category.
INTP – Female Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 29.67 | 28.33 | / 42 | 0.71 | 0.67 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.33 | 28.00 | /35 | 0.84 | 0.80 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 29.00 | 29.00 | /35 | 0.83 | 0.83 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 30.00 | 30.67 | /35 | 0.86 | 0.88 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 37.33 | 36.67 | /48 | 0.78 | 0.76 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 20.67 | 20.00 | /28 | 0.74 | 0.71 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.79333333 | 0.775 |
Variance | 0.00358667 | 0.00603 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.59480376 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.29122925 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
0.032 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.038 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.000 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.019 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
0.014 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
0.024 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.978
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: 0.0183333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0240139
Test Statistic, t: 1.8701
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.1204
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0068677 < µd < 0.0435343
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the INTP ‘Female Subjects’ category.
INTP – Male Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 29.33 | 29.17 | / 42 | 0.70 | 0.69 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.83 | 29.33 | /35 | 0.85 | 0.84 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 28.67 | 29.83 | /35 | 0.82 | 0.85 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.67 | 31.00 | /35 | 0.85 | 0.89 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 38.50 | 37.83 | /48 | 0.80 | 0.79 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.17 | 24.00 | /28 | 0.83 | 0.86 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.80833333 | 0.82 |
Variance | 0.00317667 | 0.00512 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.62044271 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.30658776 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
0.004 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.014 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.033 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.038 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
0.014 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.030 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9176
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0116667
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0240139
Test Statistic, t: -1.1900
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.2875
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0368677 < µd < 0.0135343
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the INTP ‘Male Subjects’ category.
ESTP – All Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 33.31 | 34.26 | / 42 | 0.79 | 0.82 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 27.69 | 28.68 | /35 | 0.79 | 0.82 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 28.03 | 27.98 | /35 | 0.80 | 0.80 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.58 | 29.68 | /35 | 0.85 | 0.85 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 37.49 | 38.46 | /48 | 0.78 | 0.80 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 24.13 | 23.73 | /28 | 0.86 | 0.85 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.81166667 | 0.823333333 |
Variance | 0.00117667 | 0.000506667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 2.32236842 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.18826283 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.022 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.028 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.001 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.003 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.020 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
0.014 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9709
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0116667
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.017224
Test Statistic, t: -1.6592
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.1580
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0297421 < µd < 0.0064088
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ESTP ‘All Subjects’ category.
ESTP – Female Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 31.40 | 33.20 | / 42 | 0.75 | 0.79 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 30.60 | 29.60 | /35 | 0.87 | 0.85 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 24.80 | 27.60 | /35 | 0.71 | 0.79 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.60 | 30.60 | /35 | 0.85 | 0.87 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.60 | 39.20 | /48 | 0.83 | 0.82 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 20.80 | 20.80 | /28 | 0.74 | 0.74 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.79166667 | 0.81 |
Variance | 0.00441667 | 0.0022 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 2.00757576 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.23131789 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.043 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.029 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.080 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.029 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
0.008 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
0.000 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9867
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0183333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0371035
Test Statistic, t: -1.2103
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.2802
95% Confidence interval:
-0.057271 < µd < 0.0206043
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ESTP ‘Female Subjects’ category.
ESTP – Male Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 34.14 | 34.71 | / 42 | 0.81 | 0.83 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 26.43 | 28.29 | /35 | 0.76 | 0.81 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 29.43 | 28.14 | /35 | 0.84 | 0.80 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.57 | 29.29 | /35 | 0.84 | 0.84 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 36.57 | 38.14 | /48 | 0.76 | 0.79 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 25.57 | 25.00 | /28 | 0.91 | 0.89 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.82 | 0.826666667 |
Variance | 0.00324 | 0.001306667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 2.47959184 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.17078791 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.014 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.053 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.037 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.008 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.033 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
0.020 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9932
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0066667
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0332666
Test Statistic, t: -0.4909
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.6443
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0415778 < µd < 0.0282445
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence do not support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is different from zero.
The results show that there was no significant change in the ESTP ‘Male Subjects’ category.
ESFP – All Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 32.91 | 33.46 | / 42 | 0.78 | 0.80 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.00 | 28.53 | /35 | 0.83 | 0.82 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 30.18 | 29.51 | /35 | 0.86 | 0.84 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.69 | 30.28 | /35 | 0.85 | 0.87 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 40.10 | 40.96 | /48 | 0.84 | 0.85 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.91 | 24.91 | /28 | 0.85 | 0.89 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.835 | 0.845 |
Variance | 0.00083 | 0.00107 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.77570093 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.39363316 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.013 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.013 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.019 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.017 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.018 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.035 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9577
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.01
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0219089
Test Statistic, t: -1.1180
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.3144
95% Confidence interval:
-0.032992 < µd < 0.012992
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ESFP ‘All Subjects’ category.
ESFP – Female Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 33.73 | 34.27 | / 42 | 0.80 | 0.82 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 28.73 | 28.47 | /35 | 0.82 | 0.81 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 29.07 | 28.13 | /35 | 0.83 | 0.80 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 28.73 | 27.33 | /35 | 0.82 | 0.78 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.07 | 40.60 | /48 | 0.81 | 0.85 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.20 | 23.67 | /28 | 0.83 | 0.85 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.81833333 | 0.818333333 |
Variance | 0.00013667 | 0.000776667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.17596567 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.03973302 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p < .05, reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
t-Test: Two-Sample Assuming Unequal Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.81833333 | 0.818333333 |
Variance | 0.00013667 | 0.000776667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
Hypothesized Mean Difference | 0 | |
df | 7 | |
t Stat | 8.9985E-15 | |
P(T<=t) one-tail | 0.5 | |
t Critical one-tail | 1.8945786 | |
P(T<=t) two-tail | 1 | |
t Critical two-tail | 2.36462425 |
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.013 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.008 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.027 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.040 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.032 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.017 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9827
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -1.850372e-17
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0316228
Test Statistic, t: -0.0000
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 1.0000
95% Confidence interval:
-0.033186 < µd < 0.033186
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ESFP ‘Female Subjects’ category.
ESFP – Male Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 32.56 | 33.11 | / 42 | 0.78 | 0.79 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.11 | 28.56 | /35 | 0.83 | 0.82 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 30.67 | 30.11 | /35 | 0.88 | 0.86 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 30.11 | 31.56 | /35 | 0.86 | 0.90 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 40.56 | 41.11 | /48 | 0.84 | 0.86 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 24.22 | 25.44 | /28 | 0.87 | 0.91 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.84333333 | 0.856666667 |
Variance | 0.00130667 | 0.002106667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.62025316 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.30647515 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.013 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.016 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.016 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.041 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.012 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.044 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9488
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0133333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0250333
Test Statistic, t: -1.3047
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.2488
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0396042 < µd < 0.0129375
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ESFP ‘Male Subjects’ category.
ENFP – All Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 31.79 | 30.99 | / 42 | 0.76 | 0.74 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 32.60 | 32.54 | /35 | 0.93 | 0.93 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 31.58 | 30.28 | /35 | 0.90 | 0.87 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 31.67 | 31.79 | /35 | 0.90 | 0.91 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 41.48 | 42.62 | /48 | 0.86 | 0.89 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 24.97 | 25.10 | /28 | 0.89 | 0.90 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.87333333 | 0.873333333 |
Variance | 0.00358667 | 0.004666667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.76857143 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.38987432 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
0.019 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.002 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.037 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.003 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.024 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.005 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9781
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: 0
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0219089
Test Statistic, t: 0.0000
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 1.0000
95% Confidence interval:
-0.022992 < µd < 0.022992
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ENFP ‘All Subjects’ category.
ENFP – Female Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 30.79 | 29.43 | / 42 | 0.73 | 0.70 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 31.93 | 30.71 | /35 | 0.91 | 0.88 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 31.64 | 29.64 | /35 | 0.90 | 0.85 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 30.14 | 28.50 | /35 | 0.86 | 0.81 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 37.71 | 38.93 | /48 | 0.79 | 0.81 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 24.64 | 26.36 | /28 | 0.88 | 0.94 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.845 | 0.831666667 |
Variance | 0.00499 | 0.006536667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.76338603 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.38712568 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
0.032 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.035 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.057 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.047 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.025 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.061 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9289
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: 0.0133333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.044121
Test Statistic, t: 0.7402
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.4924
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0329688 < µd < 0.0596355
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ENFP ‘Female Subjects’ category.
ENFP – Male Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 32.22 | 31.67 | / 42 | 0.77 | 0.75 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 32.89 | 33.33 | /35 | 0.94 | 0.95 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 31.56 | 30.56 | /35 | 0.90 | 0.87 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 32.33 | 33.22 | /35 | 0.92 | 0.95 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 43.11 | 44.22 | /48 | 0.90 | 0.92 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 25.11 | 24.56 | /28 | 0.90 | 0.88 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.88833333 | 0.886666667 |
Variance | 0.00361667 | 0.005626667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.64277251 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.31975223 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
0.013 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.013 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.029 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.025 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.023 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
0.020 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9538
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: 0.0016667
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0248328
Test Statistic, t: 0.1644
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.8759
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0243937 < µd < 0.027727
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ENFP ‘Male Subjects’ category.
ENTP – All Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 33.61 | 33.22 | / 42 | 0.80 | 0.79 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 23.76 | 25.33 | /35 | 0.68 | 0.72 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 28.28 | 27.75 | /35 | 0.81 | 0.79 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 28.41 | 29.13 | /35 | 0.81 | 0.83 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 37.52 | 37.29 | /48 | 0.78 | 0.78 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 19.12 | 21.29 | /28 | 0.68 | 0.76 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.76 | 0.778333333 |
Variance | 0.00396 | 0.001336667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 2.96259352 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.1291995 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
0.009 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.045 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.015 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.021 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
0.005 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.077 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9519
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0183333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0371035
Test Statistic, t: -1.2103
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.2802
95% Confidence interval:
-0.057271 < µd < 0.0206043
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ENTP ‘All Subjects’ category.
ENTP – Female Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 32.25 | 33.25 | / 42 | 0.77 | 0.79 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 25.50 | 27.00 | /35 | 0.73 | 0.77 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 28.00 | 29.00 | /35 | 0.80 | 0.83 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 27.50 | 28.50 | /35 | 0.79 | 0.81 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 41.00 | 40.25 | /48 | 0.85 | 0.84 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 21.25 | 24.25 | /28 | 0.76 | 0.87 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.78333333 | 0.818333333 |
Variance | 0.00166667 | 0.001296667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 1.28534704 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.39484026 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.024 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.043 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.029 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.029 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
0.016 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.107 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9252
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 1
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 1
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.035
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0403733
Test Statistic, t: -2.1235
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.0871
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0773691 < µd < 0.0073691
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ENTP ‘Female Subjects’ category.
ENTP – Male Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 34.20 | 33.20 | / 42 | 0.81 | 0.79 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 23.00 | 24.60 | /35 | 0.66 | 0.70 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 28.40 | 27.20 | /35 | 0.81 | 0.78 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 28.80 | 29.40 | /35 | 0.82 | 0.84 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 36.00 | 36.00 | /48 | 0.75 | 0.75 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 18.20 | 20.00 | /28 | 0.65 | 0.71 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.75 | 0.761666667 |
Variance | 0.00604 | 0.002776667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 2.17527011 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.20689303 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
0.024 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.046 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.034 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.017 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
0.000 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.064 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9854
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0116667
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0348807
Test Statistic, t: -0.8193
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.4499
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0482717 < µd < 0.0249384
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence do not support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is different from zero.
The results show that there was no significant change in the ENTP ‘Male Subjects’ category.
ESTJ – All Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 33.25 | 33.59 | / 42 | 0.79 | 0.80 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 26.87 | 27.87 | /35 | 0.77 | 0.80 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 28.16 | 28.48 | /35 | 0.80 | 0.81 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 27.92 | 28.28 | /35 | 0.80 | 0.81 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.88 | 41.05 | /48 | 0.83 | 0.86 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 21.81 | 21.17 | /28 | 0.78 | 0.76 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.795 | 0.806666667 |
Variance | 0.00043 | 0.001026667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.41883117 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.18072969 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.008 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.028 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.009 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.010 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.024 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
0.023 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9227
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 1
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0116667
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0183485
Test Statistic, t: -1.5575
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.1801
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0309222 < µd < 0.0075889
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ESTJ ‘All Subjects’ category.
ESTJ – Female Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 32.89 | 32.33 | / 42 | 0.78 | 0.77 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 30.11 | 29.56 | /35 | 0.86 | 0.84 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 30.67 | 29.11 | /35 | 0.88 | 0.83 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 27.44 | 28.00 | /35 | 0.78 | 0.80 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.44 | 41.78 | /48 | 0.82 | 0.87 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.22 | 22.33 | /28 | 0.83 | 0.80 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.825 | 0.818333333 |
Variance | 0.00167 | 0.001256667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 1.32891247 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.38131217 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
0.013 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.016 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.044 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.016 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.049 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
0.032 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9681
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: 0.0066667
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0361478
Test Statistic, t: 0.4518
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.6704
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0312681 < µd < 0.0446015
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ESTJ ‘Female Subjects’ category.
ESTJ – Male Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 33.40 | 34.13 | / 42 | 0.80 | 0.81 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 25.47 | 27.13 | /35 | 0.73 | 0.78 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 27.07 | 28.20 | /35 | 0.77 | 0.81 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 28.13 | 28.40 | /35 | 0.80 | 0.81 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 40.07 | 40.73 | /48 | 0.83 | 0.85 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 21.20 | 20.67 | /28 | 0.76 | 0.74 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.78166667 | 0.8 |
Variance | 0.00125667 | 0.00136 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.92401961 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.4665056 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.017 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.048 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.032 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.008 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.014 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
0.019 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9813
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0183333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0248328
Test Statistic, t: -1.8084
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.1303
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0443937 < µd < 0.007727
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence do not support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is different from zero.
The results show that there was no significant change in the ESTJ ‘Male Subjects’ category.
ESFJ – All Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 33.96 | 32.76 | / 42 | 0.81 | 0.78 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.62 | 30.01 | /35 | 0.85 | 0.86 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 30.54 | 30.95 | /35 | 0.87 | 0.88 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 30.06 | 31.44 | /35 | 0.86 | 0.90 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.49 | 40.93 | /48 | 0.82 | 0.85 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 22.13 | 21.53 | /28 | 0.79 | 0.77 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.83333333 | 0.84 |
Variance | 0.00098667 | 0.00284 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.34741784 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.13536411 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
0.028 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.011 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.012 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.039 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.030 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
0.021 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9671
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0066667
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0273252
Test Statistic, t: -0.5976
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.5761
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0353427 < µd < 0.0220093
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ESFJ ‘All Subjects’ category.
ESFJ – Female Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 33.64 | 31.76 | / 42 | 0.80 | 0.76 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 30.80 | 30.04 | /35 | 0.88 | 0.86 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 30.64 | 29.68 | /35 | 0.88 | 0.85 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 30.44 | 29.68 | /35 | 0.87 | 0.85 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 40.16 | 42.60 | /48 | 0.84 | 0.89 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 22.88 | 23.20 | /28 | 0.82 | 0.83 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.84833333 | 0.84 |
Variance | 0.00113667 | 0.00192 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.59201389 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.28954302 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
0.045 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.022 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.027 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.022 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.051 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.011 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9329
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: 0.0083333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.033116
Test Statistic, t: 0.6164
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.5646
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0264197 < µd < 0.0430864
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ESFJ ‘Female Subjects’ category.
ESFJ – Male Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 34.10 | 33.20 | / 42 | 0.81 | 0.79 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.10 | 30.00 | /35 | 0.83 | 0.86 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 30.50 | 31.50 | /35 | 0.87 | 0.90 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.90 | 32.20 | /35 | 0.85 | 0.92 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.20 | 40.20 | /48 | 0.82 | 0.84 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 21.80 | 20.80 | /28 | 0.78 | 0.74 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.82666667 | 0.841666667 |
Variance | 0.00098667 | 0.004576667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.21558631 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.0587693 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
0.021 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.026 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.029 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.066 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.021 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
0.036 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9672
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.015
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.03937
Test Statistic, t: -0.9333
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.3935
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0563163 < µd < 0.0263163
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence do not support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is different from zero.
The results show that there was no significant change in the ESFJ ‘Male Subjects’ category.
ENFJ – All Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 32.38 | 32.27 | / 42 | 0.77 | 0.77 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 31.89 | 31.08 | /35 | 0.91 | 0.89 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 30.23 | 30.76 | /35 | 0.86 | 0.88 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 30.98 | 31.64 | /35 | 0.89 | 0.90 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 38.20 | 39.05 | /48 | 0.80 | 0.81 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 21.91 | 22.59 | /28 | 0.78 | 0.81 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.835 | 0.843333333 |
Variance | 0.00355 | 0.002866667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 1.23837209 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.41011721 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
0.003 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.023 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.015 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.019 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.018 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.024 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9134
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0083333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.017224
Test Statistic, t: -1.1851
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.2892
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0264088 < µd < 0.0097421
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ENFJ ‘All Subjects’ category.
ENFJ – Female Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 34.40 | 35.20 | / 42 | 0.82 | 0.84 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 32.80 | 32.40 | /35 | 0.94 | 0.93 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 29.60 | 30.20 | /35 | 0.85 | 0.86 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.80 | 30.80 | /35 | 0.85 | 0.88 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.80 | 38.00 | /48 | 0.83 | 0.79 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 24.00 | 22.80 | /28 | 0.86 | 0.81 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.85833333 | 0.851666667 |
Variance | 0.00181667 | 0.002536667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.71616294 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.36152436 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.019 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.011 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.017 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.029 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
0.038 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
0.043 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9503
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: 0.0066667
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0326599
Test Statistic, t: 0.5000
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.6383
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0276077 < µd < 0.0409411
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ENFJ ‘Female Subjects’ category.
ENFJ – Male Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 31.50 | 31.00 | / 42 | 0.77 | 0.77 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 31.50 | 30.50 | /35 | 0.91 | 0.89 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 30.50 | 31.00 | /35 | 0.86 | 0.88 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 31.50 | 32.00 | /35 | 0.89 | 0.90 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 37.50 | 39.50 | /48 | 0.80 | 0.81 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 21.00 | 22.50 | /28 | 0.78 | 0.81 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.835 | 0.843333333 |
Variance | 0.00355 | 0.002866667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 1.23837209 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.41011721 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
0.012 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.029 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.014 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.014 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.042 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.054 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9834
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0083333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.017224
Test Statistic, t: -1.1851
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.2892
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0264088 < µd < 0.0097421
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence do not support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is different from zero.
The results show that there was no significant change in the ENFJ ‘Male Subjects’ category.
ENTJ – All Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 28.22 | 29.99 | / 42 | 0.67 | 0.71 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 27.29 | 28.54 | /35 | 0.78 | 0.82 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 29.30 | 28.44 | /35 | 0.84 | 0.81 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.23 | 30.77 | /35 | 0.84 | 0.88 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 35.22 | 35.38 | /48 | 0.73 | 0.74 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 24.54 | 25.07 | /28 | 0.88 | 0.90 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.79 | 0.81 |
Variance | 0.00624 | 0.0056 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 1.11428571 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.45418449 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.042 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.035 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.025 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.044 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.003 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.019 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9502
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.02
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0275681
Test Statistic, t: -1.7770
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.1357
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0489309 < µd < 0.0089309
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ENTJ ‘All Subjects’ category.
ENTJ – Female Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 29.50 | 31.50 | / 42 | 0.70 | 0.75 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.50 | 29.00 | /35 | 0.84 | 0.83 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 30.00 | 31.00 | /35 | 0.86 | 0.89 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.00 | 31.00 | /35 | 0.83 | 0.89 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 36.50 | 35.50 | /48 | 0.76 | 0.74 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 25.00 | 26.00 | /28 | 0.89 | 0.93 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.81333333 | 0.838333333 |
Variance | 0.00494667 | 0.006256667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.79062334 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.40142429 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.048 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.014 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.029 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.057 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
0.021 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.036 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9484
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.025
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0327109
Test Statistic, t: -1.8721
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.1201
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0593279 < µd < 0.0093279
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence to support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is zero.
The results indicate that there was no significant change in the ENTJ ‘Female Subjects’ category.
ENTJ – Male Subjects
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 27.67 | 29.33 | / 42 | 0.66 | 0.70 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 26.33 | 28.33 | /35 | 0.75 | 0.81 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 29.00 | 27.33 | /35 | 0.83 | 0.78 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.33 | 30.67 | /35 | 0.84 | 0.88 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 34.67 | 35.33 | /48 | 0.72 | 0.74 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 24.33 | 24.67 | /28 | 0.87 | 0.88 |
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.77833333 | 0.798333333 |
Variance | 0.00661667 | 0.005376667 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 1.23062616 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.4127075 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Normality test on differences to justify the application of a T test.
Before CBT concept exposure – After CBT concept exposure normalized differences:
-0.040 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.057 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.048 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.038 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.014 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.012 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9316
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 1
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.02
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0384708
Test Statistic, t: -1.2734
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.2589
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0603726 < µd < 0.0203726
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion:
Fail to reject the null hypothesis that the average difference equals 0.
Sample provides evidence do not support the claim that the average difference between the results before CBT concept exposure and after is different from zero.
The results show that there was no significant change in the ENTJ ‘Male Subjects’ category.
Investigation of the influence of the strength of an individual MBTI category (>75% or <25%) on the effects of CBT concept exposure for the ‘All subjects’ category
All subjects ‘before treatment – after treatment’ for normalized data:
I > 75% | I < 25% | T > 75% | T < 25% |
-0.032 | -0.019 | -0.058 | 0.013 |
-0.065 | -0.011 | -0.06 | 0.006 |
-0.081 | 0.043 | -0.064 | -0.025 |
-0.042 | 0.029 | -0.052 | -0.016 |
-0.064 | 0.006 | -0.035 | 0.023 |
-0.094 | -0.014 | -0.101 | 0.012 |
E > 75% | E < 25% | F > 75% | F < 25% |
0.004 | -0.002 | -0.04 | 0.024 |
-0.019 | -0.02 | -0.007 | 0.044 |
-0.007 | -0.033 | -0.037 | -0.047 |
-0.062 | -0.04 | 0 | 0.018 |
-0.01 | -0.01 | 0.014 | -0.057 |
0.012 | 0.036 | -0.082 | 0 |
J > 75% | J < 25% | S > 75% | S < 25% |
-0.06 | 0.013 | -0.038 | 0.03 |
-0.053 | 0.044 | -0.004 | 0.016 |
-0.092 | -0.026 | -0.024 | -0.023 |
-0.044 | 0.029 | 0.035 | 0.023 |
-0.027 | -0.055 | -0.03 | -0.051 |
-0.058 | -0.003 | -0.041 | -0.006 |
P > 75% | P < 25% | N > 75% | N < 25% |
-0.048 | 0.041 | -0.002 | 0.018 |
0 | 0.016 | -0.05 | -0.022 |
-0.042 | -0.026 | -0.069 | -0.075 |
-0.002 | 0.016 | 0.005 | -0.029 |
0.014 | -0.042 | 0.024 | -0.018 |
-0.08 | -0.052 | -0.036 | 0.033 |
Bartlett’s Test confirming homoscedasticity: | ||||||||
Significance level (%): 5 | ||||||||
Summary statistics: VariableMinimumMaximumMeanStd. deviationI > 75%-0.094-0.032-0.0630.023I < 25%-0.0190.0430.0060.025T > 75%-0.101-0.035-0.0620.022T < 25%-0.0250.0230.0020.019E > 75%-0.0620.012-0.0140.026E < 25%-0.040.036-0.0120.027F > 75%-0.0820.014-0.0250.035F < 25%-0.0570.044-0.0030.041J > 75%-0.092-0.027-0.0560.021J < 25%-0.0550.04400.036S > 75%-0.0410.035-0.0170.029S < 25%-0.0510.03-0.0020.031P > 75%-0.080.014-0.0260.036P < 25%-0.0520.041-0.0080.037N > 75%-0.0690.024-0.0210.036N < 25%-0.0750.033-0.0160.038 | ||||||||
Chi-square (Observed value) | 7.661 | |||||||
Chi-square (Critical value) | 24.996 | |||||||
DF | 15 | |||||||
p-value (one-tailed) | 0.937 | |||||||
alpha | 0.05 | |||||||
Test interpretation: | ||||||||
H0: The variances are identical. | ||||||||
Ha: At least one of the variances is different from another. | ||||||||
As the computed p-value is greater than the significance level alpha=0.05, one cannot reject the null hypothesis H0. | ||||||||
The risk to Reject the null hypothesis H0 while it is true is 93.66%. |
ANOVA test for differences:
Source: DF: SS: MS: Test Stat, F: Critical F: P-Value:
Treatment: 15 0.044391 0.002959 3.095617 1.79322 0.000553
Error: 80 0.07648 0.000956
Total: 95 0.120871
Reject the null hypothesis
Reject equality of means
Conclusion: There are differential effects within the 16 groups in the ‘All subjects’ category.
Introvert – All subjects > 75% |
Normality Test on differences of Normalized data
-0.032 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.065 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.081 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.042 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.064 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.094 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9858
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.83316667 | 0.896 |
Variance | 0.00049337 | 0.0007124 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.69254164 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.34833803 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
All | All | All | All | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 34.27 | 35.64 | / 42 | 0.816 | 0.848 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.36 | 31.64 | /35 | 0.839 | 0.904 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 28.36 | 31.18 | /35 | 0.810 | 0.891 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 30.55 | 32.00 | /35 | 0.873 | 0.914 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.82 | 42.91 | /48 | 0.830 | 0.894 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.27 | 25.91 | /28 | 0.831 | 0.925 |
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0628333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0233873
Test Statistic, t: -6.5809
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.0012
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0873768 < µd < -0.0382899
Reject the null hypothesis
Sample provides evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion: There was a differential effect from the CBT concept exposure for this group in the direction of positive change.
Introvert – All subjects < 25%
-0.019 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.011 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
0.043 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.029 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
0.006 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.014 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9526
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.83716667 | 0.831833333 |
Variance | 0.00020217 | 0.000740567 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.27298915 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.09025272 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
All | All | All | All | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 34.30 | 35.10 | / 42 | 0.817 | 0.836 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.90 | 30.30 | /35 | 0.854 | 0.866 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 29.50 | 28.00 | /35 | 0.843 | 0.800 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.40 | 28.40 | /35 | 0.840 | 0.811 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.50 | 39.20 | /48 | 0.823 | 0.817 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.70 | 24.10 | /28 | 0.846 | 0.861 |
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: 0.0053333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0256333
Test Statistic, t: 0.5096
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.6320
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0215672 < µd < 0.0322338
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion: There was not a differential effect from the CBT concept exposure for this group.
Thinking – All subjects > 75%
-0.058 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.060 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.064 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.052 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.035 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.101 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9292
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 1
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.8245 | 0.8865 |
Variance | 0.0002083 | 0.0008743 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.23824774 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.07073039 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
All | All | All | All | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 34.33 | 36.75 | / 42 | 0.817 | 0.875 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.33 | 31.42 | /35 | 0.838 | 0.898 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 28.17 | 30.42 | /35 | 0.805 | 0.869 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.42 | 31.25 | /35 | 0.840 | 0.893 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.08 | 40.75 | /48 | 0.814 | 0.849 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.33 | 26.17 | /28 | 0.833 | 0.935 |
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.062
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0220635
Test Statistic, t: -6.8832
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.0010
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0851542 < µd < -0.0388458
Reject the null hypothesis
Sample provides evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion: There was a differential effect from the CBT concept exposure for this group in the direction of positive change.
Thinking – All subjects < 25%
0.013 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.006 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.025 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.016 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
0.023 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
0.012 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9552
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.83916667 | 0.837166667 |
Variance | 0.00054057 | 0.001701367 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.31772497 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.11696471 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
All | All | All | All | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 34.11 | 33.56 | / 42 | 0.812 | 0.799 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.56 | 29.33 | /35 | 0.844 | 0.838 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 30.67 | 31.56 | /35 | 0.876 | 0.902 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.78 | 30.33 | /35 | 0.851 | 0.867 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.33 | 38.22 | /48 | 0.819 | 0.796 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.33 | 23.00 | /28 | 0.833 | 0.821 |
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: 0.002
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0188997
Test Statistic, t: 0.2592
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.8058
95% Confidence interval:
-0.017834 < µd < 0.021834
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion: There was not a differential effect from the CBT concept exposure for this group.
Judging – All subjects > 75%
-0.060 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.053 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.092 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.044 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.027 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.058 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9613
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 1
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.819 | 0.874833333 |
Variance | 0.0006392 | 0.001502967 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.4252922 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.1848669 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
All | All | All | All | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 34.00 | 36.54 | / 42 | 0.810 | 0.870 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.46 | 31.31 | /35 | 0.842 | 0.895 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 29.15 | 32.38 | /35 | 0.833 | 0.925 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 27.08 | 28.62 | /35 | 0.774 | 0.818 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.23 | 40.54 | /48 | 0.817 | 0.845 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.46 | 25.08 | /28 | 0.838 | 0.896 |
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0558333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0212266
Test Statistic, t: -6.4430
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.0013
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0781092 < µd < -0.0335575
Reject the null hypothesis
Sample provides evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion: There was a differential effect from the CBT concept exposure for this group in the direction of positive change.
Judging – All subjects < 25%
0.013 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.044 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.026 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.029 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.055 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.003 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9905
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.835 | 0.834666667 |
Variance | 0.0002096 | 0.001099467 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.19063789 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.04647222 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p < .05, reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
t-Test: Two-Sample Assuming Unequal Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.835 | 0.834666667 |
Variance | 0.0002096 | 0.001099467 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
Hypothesized Mean Difference | 0 | |
df | 7 | |
t Stat | 0.02256698 | |
P(T<=t) one-tail | 0.49131275 | |
t Critical one-tail | 1.8945786 | |
P(T<=t) two-tail | 0.98262549 | |
t Critical two-tail | 2.36462425 |
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
All | All | All | All | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 34.36 | 33.82 | / 42 | 0.818 | 0.805 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.82 | 28.27 | /35 | 0.852 | 0.808 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 29.64 | 30.55 | /35 | 0.847 | 0.873 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.18 | 28.18 | /35 | 0.834 | 0.805 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.27 | 41.91 | /48 | 0.818 | 0.873 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.55 | 23.64 | /28 | 0.841 | 0.844 |
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: 0.0003333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0364838
Test Statistic, t: 0.0224
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.9830
95% Confidence interval:
-0.037954 < µd < 0.0386207
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion: There was not a differential effect from the CBT concept exposure for this group.
Sensing – All subjects > 75%
-0.038 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.004 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.024 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.035 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.030 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.041 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9179
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.84216667 | 0.859333333 |
Variance | 0.00099657 | 0.000961067 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 1.03693812 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.48460979 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
All | All | All | All | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 34.15 | 35.77 | / 42 | 0.813 | 0.852 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 31.46 | 31.62 | /35 | 0.899 | 0.903 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 29.46 | 30.31 | /35 | 0.842 | 0.866 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.69 | 28.46 | /35 | 0.848 | 0.813 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.00 | 40.46 | /48 | 0.813 | 0.843 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.46 | 24.62 | /28 | 0.838 | 0.879 |
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0171667
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0288126
Test Statistic, t: -1.4594
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.2043
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0474036 < µd < 0.0130703
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion: There was not a differential effect from the CBT concept exposure for this group.
Sensing – All subjects < 25%
0.030 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.016 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.023 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.023 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.051 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.006 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9684
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances BeforeAfterMean0.837166670.839Variance0.000211770.0009716Observations66df55F0.21795664 P(F<=f) one-tail0.05998746 F Critical one-tail0.1980069 Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After) Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences: GroupAverage Normalized Data AllAll AllAll BeforeAfter BeforeAfterGratitude Questionnaire34.5533.27/ 420.8230.792Satisfaction with Life Scale29.3628.82/350.8390.823Meaning in Life (Presence)29.8230.64/350.8520.875Meaning in Life (Search)29.7328.91/350.8490.826Personal Growth Initiative Scale39.1841.64/480.8160.867Subjective Happiness Scale23.6423.82/280.8440.851 |
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0018333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0313204
Test Statistic, t: -0.1434
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.8916
95% Confidence interval:
-0.034702 < µd < 0.0310354
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion: There was not a differential effect from the CBT concept exposure for this group.
Extravert – All subjects > 75%
0.004 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.019 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.007 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.062 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.010 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
0.012 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9241
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 1
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.87066667 | 0.8845 |
Variance | 0.00195307 | 0.0013227 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 1.47657569 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.33966498 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
All | All | All | All | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 37.00 | 36.83 | / 42 | 0.881 | 0.877 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 30.75 | 31.42 | /35 | 0.879 | 0.898 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 29.75 | 30.00 | /35 | 0.850 | 0.857 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.42 | 31.58 | /35 | 0.840 | 0.902 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.58 | 40.08 | /48 | 0.825 | 0.835 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 26.58 | 26.25 | /28 | 0.949 | 0.938 |
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0138333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0258567
Test Statistic, t: -1.3105
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.2470
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0409682 < µd < 0.0133015
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion: There was not a differential effect from the CBT concept exposure for this group.
Extravert – All subjects < 25%
-0.002 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.020 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.033 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.040 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.010 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
0.036 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9588
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.85883333 | 0.870333333 |
Variance | 0.00286457 | 0.001653067 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 1.7328803 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.28046513 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
All | All | All | All | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 36.62 | 36.69 | / 42 | 0.872 | 0.874 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.54 | 30.23 | /35 | 0.844 | 0.864 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 31.54 | 32.69 | /35 | 0.901 | 0.934 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 27.31 | 28.69 | /35 | 0.780 | 0.820 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.69 | 40.15 | /48 | 0.827 | 0.837 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 26.00 | 25.00 | /28 | 0.929 | 0.893 |
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0115
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0271864
Test Statistic, t: -1.0361
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.3476
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0400303 < µd < 0.0170303
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion: There was not a differential effect from the CBT concept exposure for this group.
Feeling – All subjects > 75%
-0.040 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.007 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.037 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.000 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
0.014 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.082 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9689
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.83383333 | 0.859 |
Variance | 0.00184457 | 0.0034352 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.53696049 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.25571991 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
All | All | All | All | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 34.31 | 36.00 | / 42 | 0.817 | 0.857 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 31.00 | 31.23 | /35 | 0.886 | 0.892 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 31.00 | 32.31 | /35 | 0.886 | 0.923 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 27.31 | 27.31 | /35 | 0.780 | 0.780 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.00 | 38.31 | /48 | 0.813 | 0.798 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.00 | 25.31 | /28 | 0.821 | 0.904 |
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0251667
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0355495
Test Statistic, t: -1.7341
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.1434
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0624735 < µd < 0.0121402
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion: There was not a differential effect from the CBT concept exposure for this group.
Feeling – All subjects < 25%
0.024 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.044 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.047 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.018 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.057 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
0.000 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9615
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.83 | 0.832833333 |
Variance | 0.0001744 | 0.001032567 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.16889951 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.03663141 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p < .05, reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
t-Test: Two-Sample Assuming Unequal Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.83 | 0.832833333 |
Variance | 0.0001744 | 0.001032567 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
Hypothesized Mean Difference | 0 | |
df | 7 | |
t Stat | -0.1997679 | |
P(T<=t) one-tail | 0.42367073 | |
t Critical one-tail | 1.8945786 | |
P(T<=t) two-tail | 0.84734146 | |
t Critical two-tail | 2.36462425 |
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
All | All | All | All | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 34.82 | 33.82 | / 42 | 0.829 | 0.805 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.82 | 28.27 | /35 | 0.852 | 0.808 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 29.09 | 30.73 | /35 | 0.831 | 0.878 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.18 | 28.55 | /35 | 0.834 | 0.816 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.00 | 41.73 | /48 | 0.813 | 0.869 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.00 | 23.00 | /28 | 0.821 | 0.821 |
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0028333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0403307
Test Statistic, t: -0.1721
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.8701
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0451578 < µd < 0.0394911
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion: There was not a differential effect from the CBT concept exposure for this group.
Perceiving – All subjects > 75%
-0.048 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.000 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.042 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.002 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
0.014 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.080 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.965
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.83966667 | 0.866 |
Variance | 0.00173387 | 0.0033388 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.51930833 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.2446784 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
All | All | All | All | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 34.15 | 36.15 | / 42 | 0.813 | 0.861 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 31.15 | 31.15 | /35 | 0.890 | 0.890 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 31.08 | 32.54 | /35 | 0.888 | 0.930 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 27.54 | 27.62 | /35 | 0.787 | 0.789 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.46 | 38.77 | /48 | 0.822 | 0.808 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.46 | 25.69 | /28 | 0.838 | 0.918 |
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0263333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0360758
Test Statistic, t: -1.7880
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.1338
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0641926 < µd < 0.0115259
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion: There was not a differential effect from the CBT concept exposure for this group.
Perceiving – All subjects < 25%
0.041 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
0.016 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.026 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.016 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.042 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.052 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.965
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.837 | 0.844833333 |
Variance | 0.0001396 | 0.001914167 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.07292991 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.00607365 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p < .05, reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
t-Test: Two-Sample Assuming Unequal Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.837 | 0.844833333 |
Variance | 0.0001396 | 0.001914167 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
Hypothesized Mean Difference | 0 | |
df | 6 | |
t Stat | -0.4233959 | |
P(T<=t) one-tail | 0.34338174 | |
t Critical one-tail | 1.94318027 | |
P(T<=t) two-tail | 0.68676347 | |
t Critical two-tail | 2.44691185 |
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
All | All | All | All | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 34.73 | 33.00 | / 42 | 0.827 | 0.786 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.45 | 28.91 | /35 | 0.842 | 0.826 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 29.73 | 30.64 | /35 | 0.849 | 0.875 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.00 | 28.45 | /35 | 0.829 | 0.813 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.55 | 41.55 | /48 | 0.824 | 0.866 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.82 | 25.27 | /28 | 0.851 | 0.903 |
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0078333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0373332
Test Statistic, t: -0.5140
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.6292
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0470121 < µd < 0.0313454
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion: There was not a differential effect from the CBT concept exposure for this group.
Intuition – All subjects > 75%
-0.002 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.050 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.069 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
0.005 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
0.024 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
-0.036 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9829
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.83316667 | 0.8545 |
Variance | 0.00038617 | 0.0014943 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 0.25842647 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.08191473 | |
F Critical one-tail | 0.1980069 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
All | All | All | All | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 36.08 | 36.17 | / 42 | 0.859 | 0.861 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 29.42 | 31.17 | /35 | 0.840 | 0.890 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 28.25 | 30.67 | /35 | 0.807 | 0.876 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 29.00 | 28.83 | /35 | 0.829 | 0.824 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.17 | 38.00 | /48 | 0.816 | 0.792 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 23.75 | 24.75 | /28 | 0.848 | 0.884 |
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0213333
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0358646
Test Statistic, t: -1.4570
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.2049
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0589708 < µd < 0.0163042
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion: There was not a differential effect from the CBT concept exposure for this group.
Intuition – All subjects < 25%
0.018 | Gratitude Questionnaire |
-0.022 | Satisfaction with Life Scale |
-0.075 | Meaning in Life (Presence) |
-0.029 | Meaning in Life (Search) |
-0.018 | Personal Growth Initiative Scale |
0.033 | Subjective Happiness Scale |
Ryan-Joiner test for normality:
Test statistic, Rp (correlation coefficient): 0.9721
Critical value for 0.01 significance level: 0.841
Fail to reject normality with a 0.01 significance level.
Possible Outliers
Number of data values below Q1 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Number of data values above Q3 by more than 1.5 INTERQUARTILE RANGE: 0
Conclusion: The normality test justifies the application of the following mean-matched pairs T-test to determine differential before/after CBT concept exposure effects.
F-Test Two-Sample for Variances | ||
Before | After | |
Mean | 0.8495 | 0.864666667 |
Variance | 0.0025403 | 0.002079067 |
Observations | 6 | 6 |
df | 5 | 5 |
F | 1.22184634 | |
P(F<=f) one-tail | 0.41566845 | |
F Critical one-tail | 5.05032906 |
Since p >.05, fail to reject claim: variance (Before) = variance (After)
Mean-matched pairs T-test on normalized differences:
Group | Average | Normalized | Data | ||
All | All | All | All | ||
Before | After | Before | After | ||
Gratitude Questionnaire | 34.77 | 34.00 | / 42 | 0.828 | 0.810 |
Satisfaction with Life Scale | 30.62 | 31.38 | /35 | 0.875 | 0.897 |
Meaning in Life (Presence) | 29.38 | 32.00 | /35 | 0.840 | 0.914 |
Meaning in Life (Search) | 27.77 | 28.77 | /35 | 0.793 | 0.822 |
Personal Growth Initiative Scale | 39.54 | 40.38 | /48 | 0.824 | 0.841 |
Subjective Happiness Scale | 26.23 | 25.31 | /28 | 0.937 | 0.904 |
Claim: µ(before) – µ(after) = 0
Sample size, n: 6
Difference Mean, d: -0.0151667
Difference St Dev, sd: 0.0377434
Test Statistic, t: -0.9843
Critical t: ±2.5706
P-Value: 0.3702
95% Confidence interval:
-0.0547759 < µd < 0.0244426
Fail to reject the null hypothesis
Sample does not provide enough evidence to reject the claim
Conclusion: There was not a differential effect from the CBT concept exposure for this group.
1 The following exceptional cases occurred in relation to the two sample F-test for variances (p< .05) which necessitated a two-sample t-test assuming unequal variances before attempting a mean-matched pairs test: ISTP – Male Subjects, ESFP – Female Subjects, Judging – All subjects < 25%, Feeling – All subjects < 25%, and Perceiving – All subjects < 25%. In all of these cases the results of the two-sample t-test assuming unequal variances yielded the same results with respect to hypothesis conclusion as the mean-matched pairs T-test on differences assuming equal variances. Both results were included in the exploration of these cases (Appendix F).
2 Acceptance and Commitment Therapy, Adlerian Psychotherapy, Anger Management, Applied Behavior Analysis, Art Therapy, Autogenic Training, Aversion Therapy, Animal Assisted Psychotherapy, Bibliotherapy, Bioenergetic Analysis, Biofeedback, Brainspotting, Breathwork; Clean Language, Symbolic, Modeling and the Metaphor Therapy, Coherence Therapy, Collaborative Couple Therapy, Complementary and Alternative Medicine (CAM), Conflict Resolution Therapy, Contemplative Psychotherapy, Continuum, Core Energetics, Critical Incidence Stress Debriefing, Dance and Movement Therapy, Depth Hypnosis, Depth Therapy, Dialectical Behavior Therapy, Drama Therapy, Dream Analysis, Dyadic Developmental Psychotherapy, Ecotherapy, Ego State Therapy, Emotion Focused Therapy, Emotional Freedom Technique, Emotional Transformation Therapy (ETT), Energy Psychology, Equine and Animal-Assisted Psychotherapies, Existential Psychotherapy, Exposure Therapy, Expressive Arts Therapy, Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing Therapy (EMDR), Family Attachment Narrative Therapy, Family Systems Therapy, Feldenkrais Method, Feminist Therapy, Filial Therapy, Gestalt Therapy, Guided Therapeutic Imagery, Hakomi Experiential Psychotherapy, Healing Touch, Holographic Memory Resolution, Holotropic Breathwork, Hypnotherapy, Imago Relationship Therapy, Integral Psychotherapy, Integrative Body Psychotherapy, Interpersonal Neurobiology, Journal Therapy, Journey Therapy, Logotherapy, Middendorf Breath Experience, Mindfulness Based Cognitive Therapy, Motivational Enhancement Therapy, Music Therapy, Narrative Therapy, Neuro-Linguistic Programming, Neurofeedback, Neuropsychology, Nonviolent Communication, Parent-Child Interaction Therapy (PCIT), Rogerian Therapy, Play Therapy, Poetry Therapy, Positive Psychotherapy, Possibility Therapy, Experiential Therapy for Couples, Process Oriented Psychology, Psycho-Physical Therapy, Psychodrama, Psychodynamic Therapy, Psychosynthesis, Radiant Heart Therapy, Reality Therapy, Redecision Therapy, Regression Therapy, Reichian Breathwork, Relational Psychotherapy, Relational Recovery, Relationship Enhancement Therapy, Rubenfeld Synergy Methods, Sand Tray Therapy, Satir Transformational Systemic Therapy, Schema Therapy, Self Psychology, Self-Acceptance Training, Sensorimotor Psychotherapy, Shamanic Journeying, Somatic Psychotherapy, Soul-Centered Psychiatry, Fitness Psychology, Theraplay, Thought Field Therapy, Time Line Therapy, Transactional Analysis, Transpersonal Psychotherapy, Trauma-Focused Cognitive Behavioral Therapy, Voice Dialogue, Voice Therapy, Wilderness Therapy, and Yoga Therapy (Boterhoven De Haan & Lee, 2014; Carrere, 2013; Castonguay, 2013; Connors, 2011; Cross, 2013; Elkins, 2012; Feldstein Ewing & Chung, 2013, 2013; Gibson, 2011; Gold, 2011; Gottdiener, 2013; Hinshelwood, 2012; Hobson, 2012; Kalawski, 2011; Lambert, 2013; Lees, 2013; Levitt & Piazza-Bonin, 2011; Martindale & Smith, 2011; Mason, 2012; McLeod, 2011; Messer, 2012; Newton, 2013; Nolan, 2013; Owen, 2013; Paris, 2013; Pienkos & Sass, 2012; Pratchett, Daly, Bierer, & Yehuda, 2011; Rabinovich & Kacen, 2011; Safran, 2013; Siddique, 2012; Slavin-Mulford, 2013; Stewart-Sicking, 2013; Tryon & Winograd, 2011; Tryon, 2013; Wampold, 2012; Wiggins, Elliott, & Cooper, 2012; Yakeley & Wood, 2011).